Which of the following statements is correct regarding vervet monkey alarm calls?

Introduction

A robust finding in animal behaviour research is that many social species produce acoustically distinct alarm calls to different predators. In primates, the phenomenon has first been studied systematically in East African vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops) with evidence for acoustically distinct alarm calls to martial eagles, leopards and pythons (Struhsaker 1967) and corresponding, adaptive anti-predator responses in listeners (Seyfarth et al. 1980). Similar findings have been reported in a range of other species, such as Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana, Zuberbühler et al. 1997), Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli, Zuberbühler 2001), putty-nosed monkeys (Cercopithecus nictitans, Arnold and Zuberbühler 2006), sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus atys, Range and Fischer 2004), lemurs (Eulemur fulvus rufus and Propithecus verreauxi, Fichtel and Kappeler 2002), meerkats (Suricata suricatta, Manser 2001) and many bird species (Suzuki 2012; Grieves et al. 2014; Cunningham and Magrath 2017). In some animal species, callers respond to dangers with complex series of alarm calls that encode information at the sequence level (Schel et al. 2010). For example, black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons) produce two types of alarm calls in long sequences that refer to both predator type and location (Berthet et al. 2019), a phenomenon also found in non-primate species (meerkats, Rauber 2020; Japanese great tits (Parus major), Suzuki 2014; pied babblers (Turdoides bicolor), Engesser et al. 2016). In some species, predator alarm calls strongly resemble aggressive calls given to conspecifics, suggesting that listeners consider contextual information to infer the cause of an event (titi monkeys, Cäsar et al. 2012; lemurs, Fichtel and Kappeler 2002; Fichtel and van Schaik 2006; saddleback tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis), Kirchhof and Hammerschmidt 2006; vervet monkeys, Price et al. 2015).

Alarm calls are usually an integral part of a more general anti-predator strategy, such as mobbing behaviour (Baker and Becker 2002; Graw and Manser 2007; Griesser 2009; Templeton 2005), predator deterrence (Caro 1986; Zuberbühler et al. 1999) or cryptic behaviour (Shalter 1978; Klump et al. 1986), and they can also play an important role in mixed-species associations (Magrath et al. 2015). In forest primates, for example, different species can assemble in large mixed-species groups, which increases the number of sentinels in visually difficult habitats beyond the species-specific group size limits (Cords 1990; Wolters and Zuberbühler 2003). Presumably, individuals of mixed-species associations know each other individually, not only within but also between species, and there is solid evidence that they understand each other’s alarm calls (Zuberbühler 2000, 2002; Rainey et al. 2004; Candiotti et al. 2012; Coye et al. 2015; Magrath et al. 2015).

In primates, mixed-species groups are often semi-permanent (Struhsaker 1981; Mitani 1991; Chapman and Chapman 1996, 2000), which is facilitated by the fact that different species exploit unique niches and thus minimize interspecies competition (Buzzard 2010). In the Taï Forest (Ivory Coast), up to eight monkey species regularly form mixed-species associations (McGraw et al. 2007). Amongst them, Diana monkeys typically forage in the highest canopy parts with a fruit-rich diet, whereas Campbell’s monkeys have a more generalist diet and exploit levels below the main canopy while lesser spot-nosed monkeys (Cercopithecus petaurista) prefer foliage in the lower levels with a large proportion of leaves in their diet (McGraw 2000; Wolters and Zuberbühler 2003; Kane and McGraw 2017; McGraw and Daegling 2020). These three guenon species spend the majority of their time in association (75–88%, Buzzard 2010).

Although all Taï Forest monkeys suffer from predation by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), leopards (Panthera pardus), crowned eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus) and humans (Boesch and Boesch 1989; Jenny 2005), foraging close to the ground is probably most dangerous due to high risks of sudden and close predator encounters (Mourthé et al. 2007). Even attacks from aerial predators are common in the lower strata of the canopy, as crowned eagles have been observed waiting for approaching monkeys within the canopy in order to launch themselves onto individuals passing underneath (Shultz 2008).

While polyspecific associations provide well-documented antipredator benefits (see Stensland et al. 2003 for review), they also require compromising in travel and time budget (Heymann 2011) and coordination in anti-predator responses, including alarm calling (Suzuki 2016), a potentially costly behaviour that attracts attention to the caller (Sherman 1977; Goodale et al. 2019). This is especially true for male forest guenons that have evolved loud alarm calls that transmit over long distances in dense vegetation, presumably due to a secondary, sexually selected function of dissuading sexual rivals (Zuberbühler 2004). This leads to the prediction that male forest monkeys should economise their alarm calling efforts whenever possible. Indeed, in Diana monkeys, males wait to produce their alarm calls until the females have started giving predator-specific alarm calls (Stephan and Zuberbühler 2016). Similarly, in Thomas langurs (Presbytis thomasi), males stop giving alarm calls as soon as other group members have responded with their own alarm calls (Wich and de Vries 2006), demonstrating that male alarm calling can be socially coordinated and that callers try to economise call production.

Whether similar social dynamics take place in mixed-species associations has, to our knowledge, never been systematically investigated. In this study, we focus on lesser spot-nosed monkeys and their most frequent association partners in the Taï Forest, Campbell’s monkeys (McGraw et al. 2007). Both species are arboreal and live in groups of one fully adult male and several adult females with their offspring (Rowe 1996). Both are comparably cryptic primates that are difficult to locate because of their inconspicuous coloration as well as their very soft and infrequent contact calls, a likely adaptation to their foraging niche in the lower forest strata (McGraw et al. 2007). Overall, this cryptic lifestyle appears to provide both species with a considerable adaptive advantage, especially in dealing with hunting pressure from chimpanzees and humans (Boesch and Boesch 1989; Boesch 1994; Covey and McGraw 2014). Indeed, both species are less threatened by extinction than the more conspicuous Western red colobus (Piliocolobus badius) and Diana monkeys, which suffer from intense levels of human poaching and are IUCN-listed as endangered (Matsuda Goodwin et al. 2017, 2019; Koné et al. 2019; McGraw et al. 2020).

Lesser spot-nosed monkey anti-predator strategies have received little attention in the literature so far. When disturbed by human presence, males produce head-bob displays and a purring alarm call, in response to which females and offspring retreat (McGraw 2000; Rowe 1996). Early reports have suggested that male alarm calls contain subtle, context-specific acoustic variability (Kambiré 2015; Kouassi 2008), similar to what has been described for Barbary macaque (Macaca Sylvanus) or chamca baboon (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) alarm calls (Fischer et al. 2001, 2010). Meaningful acoustic variations have also been reported in other species’ alarm calls, such as Siberian jay (Perisoreus infaustus) alarm calls to different predator behaviours (perched, searching or attacking hawks; Griesser 2008), titi monkey alarm calls to different predator locations (Berthet et al. 2019), black-capped chickadee (Poecile atricapillus) alarm calls to different predator sizes (Templeton 2005) and meerkat, Campbell’s monkey and blue monkey (Cercopithecus mitis) alarm calls to different event urgencies and predator types (Lemasson et al. 2010; Townsend et al. 2012; Murphy et al. 2013).

Field observation from McGraw et al. (2007) suggested that lesser spot-nosed monkeys produce a loud call in direct response to Campbell’s monkey ‘boom’, a short and low-pitched call produced by the air-sacs typically in pairs, usually in non-predatory contexts such as falling trees, group movements or neighbouring encounters (Zuberbühler 2002; Ouattara et al. 2009a). However, the function of this behaviour remains unknown. Oates (1985) described a loud call from a male lesser spot-nosed monkey as a “…loud, explosive burst of broad-band energy, followed by a train of quieter, low-pitched (energy concentrated around 0.6 kHz), closely-spaced ‘urr’ units” (p. 32). Aside from such anecdotal information, lesser spot-nosed monkey vocal behaviour has not yet been explored systematically, in contrast to the other Taï guenons (Zuberbühler et al. 1997; Eckardt and Zuberbühler 2004; Ouattara et al. 2009b; Schlenker et al. 2016).

We used playback experiments to document male lesser spot-nosed monkey vocal responses when encountering three types of dangers: eagles, leopards and falling trees. Specifically, we investigated whether male vocal responses held acoustic differences (i.e., whether there were context-specific alarm calls or call variants) and whether there was variation in the effort of production (number of calls, rate, latency) according to the type of danger encountered. We also analysed whether Campbell’s monkey alarm calling behaviour influenced the call production effort of lesser spot-nosed monkey. Finally, we explored male lesser spot-nosed monkey vocal response to three Campbell’s monkey alarm call types typically produced towards eagles, leopards and falling trees.

Method

Study site

The study was conducted in the Taï National Park (Ivory Coast), about 25 km southeast of the town of Taï. Data were collected in an approximately 50 km2 study area surrounding the ‘Centre de Recherche en Ecologie’ (5° 50′ N, 7° 21′ W) field station by KZ (1994–2002) and by ALF during a second field period (July–December 2017).

Data collection and materials

Data collection consisted of eliciting and recording alarm calls in mixed-species groups of monkeys by simulating natural disturbances with different playback stimuli. We analysed the vocal responses of male lesser spot-nosed monkeys, a species that lives in small groups with stable home ranges of about 0.7 km2 (McGraw et al. 2007). Like other Taï monkeys, lesser spot-nosed monkeys are hunted by crowned eagles, leopards, chimpanzees and humans (McGraw and Zuberbühler 2008). Non-predatory dangers are accidental bites by stationary snakes (Foerster 2008), such as Gaboon vipers (Bitis gabonica) that usually feed on smaller prey (Penner et al. 2008), falling trees or large branches (Vooren 1986) and occasional lightning strikes, which can lead to fatalities in primates and other animals.

We located mixed-species groups, mainly by listening to the conspicuous vocalisations of Diana monkeys. We recorded their location (GPS coordinates with a Garmin GPS map 62 s or estimated position on a customised grid map; see Online Resource 1, Fig. S1) and monitored their vocal behaviour for up to 30 min to ensure that no alarm calls were given while approaching to about 25–50 m (outside visual range). To keep data points independent, we took into account the home range of lesser spot-nosed monkeys (approximately 0.7 km2; McGraw et al. 2007) and the natural occurrence of leopard and crowned eagle encounters, male Campbell’s monkey alarm calling and falling tree events (Vooren 1986; Zuberbühler and Jenny 2002; McGraw et al. 2007). Based on these data and considerations, we only considered trials if groups were located at least 950 m (GPS location) or 1200 m (grid map) apart and if trials with the same stimulus were separated by at least 30 days.

Vocal responses were elicited by simulating the presence of danger, i.e., by playing back 14 s of crowned eagle shrieks, 14 s of leopard growls or 7 s of the thundering sound of a falling tree. Campbell’s monkey playbacks consisted of series of ‘krak’ alarms (produced in the presence of a leopard), ‘hok’ alarms (produced in the presence of an eagle), or ‘boom’ calls (produced in non-predatory contexts such as falling trees) (Ouattara et al. 2009a). See Online Resource 1, Table S1 for details on playback stimuli. Campbell’s monkey playbacks of ‘krak’, ‘hok’ and ‘boom’ series were used to provoke matching alarm call responses in Diana monkeys (Zuberbühler 2000, 2002), suggesting that their meanings can be transmitted across species. It was not possible to record data blindly because our study involved animals in the field.

ALF played back sounds from an iPhone 3GS connected to an Alpha speaker (AER, The Acoustic People; frequency range 60–18,000 Hz). KZ played back sounds from a Sony WMD6C Professional Walkman connected to a Nagra DSM speaker-amplifier (frequency response 60–15,000 Hz ± 4 dB). Vocalisations were tape-recorded with a Sony TCM5000EV cassette recorder and a Sennheiser ME88 or ME67 directional microphone (frequency response, 40–20,000 Hz ± 2.5 dB; KZ) or a Marantz 661 solid-state recorder (44.1 kHz sampling rate, 16 bits accuracy, WAV format) and a Sennheiser ME67 directional microphone (frequency response, 40–20,000 Hz ± 2.5 dB; ALF). Prior to analysis, all cassette recordings were digitised (44.1 kHz sampling rate, 16 bits accuracy, WAV format) with Audacity 2.1.0 using a TASCAM CD-A500 and a Technics M280 cassette player connected via a 3.5 mm jack to 2 × RCA phono stereo audio cable to a desktop computer.

Call coding

We analysed the vocalisations produced during the 2 min following an eagle, leopard or falling tree playback to explore the overall response pattern of male lesser spot-nosed monkeys. Regarding playbacks of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls, we investigated the males’ immediate responses, i.e., vocalisations produced during the first 30 s following playbacks. In addition, we recorded any naturally occurring lesser spot-nosed and Campbell’s monkeys male alarm calls to support the call classification analyses and for graphical representations (see Online Resource 1, Table S2).

Male lesser spot-nosed and Campbell’s monkeys’ vocal responses were coded by ALF using the acoustic software Raven Pro 1.5.0 (FFT size 1024, Hann window, hop size 6.96 ms). For lesser spot-nosed monkeys, we discriminated three main call types, ‘kroo’, ‘tcha’ and ‘kow’, by visual inspection of spectrograms and by ear. Campbell’s monkey vocal responses to playbacks were coded using spectrograms and audio samples from previous studies (Zuberbühler 2001; Ouattara et al. 2009a, b). For each call, ALF coded the call type, start and end time, quality (good/bad, depending on interference and background noise), species identity (lesser spot-nosed or Campbell’s monkeys) and trial ID. A second observer (QG) recoded 20% of the lesser spot-nosed monkey vocal dataset and confirmed the reliability of these classifications (Cohen’s Kappa, k = 0.85; ‘irr’ R package v0.84.1; Gamer et al. 2019).

Raven Pro 1.5.0 was set to automatically extract acoustic parameters after ALF manually selected calls by inspecting their spectrograms (see Kershenbaum et al. 2016). The following acoustic parameters were extracted for both male lesser spot-nosed and Campbell’s monkeys: delta time (i.e., call duration in s), average entropy (i.e., the amount of disorder for a typical spectrum slice of a call in bits), aggregate entropy (i.e., the overall disorder of a call in bits), peak frequency (i.e., the maximum frequency measured on a call in kHz), peak time relative (i.e., the relative position of the peak frequency on a call (measure between 0 and 1)), low frequency (i.e., the lowest frequency of a call in kHz), high frequency (i.e., the highest frequency of a call in kHz), delta frequency (i.e., the frequency range between ‘low frequency’ and ‘high frequency’ in kHz) and centre time relative (i.e., the relative position of the point where a call is divided into two parts of equal energy (measure between 0 and 1)).

For the subsequent call classification, we selected only good quality calls (i.e., free of interference and with low background noise). We replaced outliers by the means (for details on outlier identification and sample sizes, see Online Resource 1, Table S3). We then removed highly correlated acoustic parameters (Pearson correlation test; coefficient > 0.8) and kept the following ones: delta time, average entropy, peak time relative, peak frequency, low frequency, high frequency and centre time relative. All non-correlated acoustic parameters were standardized prior to analysis.

Call classification

We carried out a permutated discriminant function analysis (pDFA; R function by Roger Mundry, based on the lda function of the ‘MASS’ R package version 7.5–53; Venables and Ripley 2002; Mundry and Sommer 2007) to check the classification in the three attributed call types (i.e., ‘kroo’, ‘tcha’, ‘kow’) with call type as test variable and the trial ID as control factor (pDFA1). The pDFA allows to control for the non-independence of data due to repeated measurements (i.e., control factor), unlike conventional DFAs, and is now commonly used for acoustic analyses of animal vocal repertoires (e.g., Collier et al. 2020; Keenan et al. 2020). We also analysed inter-individual variation by running another pDFA (pDFA2) on N = 6 different males (i.e., six different trials, selected based on home range estimates and time of recording; see Online Resource 1, Table S4), who produced a minimum of 22 ‘kroo’ calls of good acoustic quality (range: 22–55 calls). For this analysis, we used the trial ID as the test variable and the playback stimulus as a control factor (i.e., type of danger or type of Campbell’s monkey calls; see Online Resource 1, Table S4). The two call types ‘tcha’ and ‘kow’ were almost always given as combinations consisting of one ‘tcha’ followed by one ‘kow’ (mean time interval between two calls = 0.09 s, SD = 0.04 s, range = 0.02–0.19 s). We recorded a total of N = 90 combinations of ‘tcha’ followed by ‘kow’, N = 4 single ‘tcha’, and N = 9 single ‘kow’, so we decided to analyse them as one combined call (i.e., ‘tcha-kow’). Single ‘tcha’ and ‘kow’ were produced across contexts and, due to small sample sizes, we were unable to investigate their production further in this study.

Responses to dangers

To test if ‘kroos’ contained subtle, context-specific acoustic variation, we first explored whether this alarm call type could be discriminated depending on the type of danger. We ran a pDFA (pDFA3) with the ‘kroos’ produced in response to the leopard growls (N = 160 ‘kroos’ over N = 12 playback trials), eagle shrieks (N = 60 ‘kroos’ over N = 4 playback trials) and falling trees (N = 120 ‘kroos’ over N = 5 playback trials). To avoid ambiguity, we only considered playback trials without Campbell’s monkey vocal response. We used the type of danger (leopard/eagle/tree) as test variable and the trial ID as control factor.

We ran several generalized linear models (GLMs) to explore the effects of playback stimuli (i.e., leopard, eagle, falling tree) and Campbell’s monkey vocal responses on lesser spot-nosed monkey call production. For the following GLMs (GLM1 to 5), we used the interaction between the type of danger (leopard/eagle/falling tree) and the Campbell’s monkey vocal responses (yes/no) as test variables. We first tested the response probability by running a GLM (GLM1) with a binomial error structure and logit link function, with the vocal response (yes/no) as response variable. We then analysed the production of the specific call types (‘kroo’ and ‘tcha-kow’) and thus only considered trials with vocal responses to test which call type was produced first. We tested this by running a GLM (GLM2) with a binomial error structure and logit link function, with the first call type (‘kroo’/‘tcha-kow’) as response variable. Since ‘kroos’ were produced in all contexts, we explored what factors determined calling effort. We tested this by running a GLM (GLM3) with a negative binomial error structure and a log link function, with the total number of ‘kroo’ produced during the trial as response variable. For this analysis, we excluded trials where the male Campbell’s monkey started to vocalise after the male lesser spot-nosed monkey produced his first ‘kroo’ to avoid ambiguity. In another GLM (GLM4) with a gamma distribution and inverse link function, we analysed the initial call rate (three first ‘kroos’ per time in s) as response variable. Here again, we excluded trials where the male Campbell’s monkey started to vocalise between the male lesser spot-nosed monkey first and third ‘kroo’ to avoid ambiguity during call production. In a final GLM (GLM5) with a gamma distribution and inverse link function, we analysed the response latency (s) as response variable, determined as the time interval between the beginning of the stimulus and the first ‘kroo’ response.

Responses to Campbell’s monkey alarm calls

We ran a second series of analyses, analogous to the previous ones, this time with the male Campbell’s monkeys alarm calls as playback stimuli (‘hok’, ‘krak’ and ‘boom’). We controlled for variations within each playback type (i.e., number of calls, intermediate intervals; Online Resource, Table S1) by using generalised linear mixed models (GLMMs) with GLMM1-GLMM5 corresponding to previous GLM1 to 5, with the same distributions and response variables, respectively. We used the Campbell’s monkey alarm call playbacks (‘hok’/ ‘krak’/ ‘boom’) as test variables and the playback variation as random factor for all GLMMs. To prevent a singular model fit in GLMM1, we used the bglmer function of the ‘blme’ R package version 1.0–5 (Chung et al. 2013) with a null covariance prior and a normal fixed effect prior.

For all GLMs and GLMMs, the significance of the tested variables was established using the Anova function in the ‘car’ R package version 3.0–10 (Fox and Weisberg 2019). When the interaction between the type of danger and the Campbell’s monkey vocal response (for the GLMs) or the Campbell’s monkey alarm playbacks (for the GLMMs) was significant, we carried out Tukey post hoc multiple comparison tests using the function glht of the ‘multcomp’ R package version 1.4–15 (Hothorn et al. 2008) to see which combinations were significantly different from each other. Model assumptions were checked using Q-Q and residual plots.

All analyses were done in R v1.3.1093 (R Core Team 2020).

Results

Call classification

Male lesser spot-nosed monkeys produced three distinct calls, ‘kroo’, ‘tcha’ and ‘kow’, and the pDFA1 confirmed that the calls could be reliably classified using our set of acoustic parameters (N = 772 ‘kroos’, N = 50 ‘tchas’, N = 54 ‘kows’, p = 0.002, correctly cross-classified = 77%, expected = 35%; Fig. 1). However, we found no pDFA evidence for inter-individual acoustic variation in ‘kroo’ production (pDFA2, N = 204 ‘kroos’ over N = 6 males lesser spot-nosed monkeys, p = 0.52). We also found no pDFA evidence for context-specific acoustic variation, regardless of the type of danger (leopard/eagle/falling tree; pDFA3; N = 345 ‘kroos’, p = 0.55; Fig. 2).

Fig. 1

Which of the following statements is correct regarding vervet monkey alarm calls?

Male lesser spot-nosed monkey ‘kroo’ (yellow), ‘tcha’ (pink) and ‘kow’ (blue) calls, plotted in principal component space with corresponding spectrograms. PC1 and PC2 accounted for 57.4% of variance. Principal component analyses were calculated with PCA function from the ‘FactoMineR’ R package 2.3 (Lê et al. 2008); spectrograms were extracted from Raven Pro 1.5.0 and contrasted with Adobe Photoshop 21.0.3

Full size image

Fig. 2

Which of the following statements is correct regarding vervet monkey alarm calls?

Male Campbell’s and lesser spot-nosed monkey alarm calls to eagles, leopards and falling trees, plotted in principal component space. PC1 and PC2 accounted for 63.5% of variance and segregate the three Campbell’s monkey alarm calls, but not the lesser spot-nosed monkey ‘kroo’ alarm calls

Full size image

Responses to dangers

We ran N = 65 playback trials on lesser spot-nosed monkey groups to investigate how males used their calls (i.e., ‘kroo’ and combined ‘tcha-kow’ calls) in response to common dangers (eagle: N = 19; leopard: N = 31; falling trees: N = 15). We found that, if Campbell’s monkeys did not alarm call first, males only produced ‘kroos’, regardless of the type of simulated danger (Fig. 3a). Conversely, if Campbell’s monkeys gave alarm calls first, males produced either ‘kroos’ to all three dangers or ‘tcha-kows’ to eagles and falling trees and exceptionally both ‘kroos’ and ‘tcha-kows’ to falling trees (Fig. 3a).

Fig. 3

Which of the following statements is correct regarding vervet monkey alarm calls?

Male lesser spot-nosed monkey a call production, b first call produced, c number of ‘kroo’ produced, d initial rate of ‘kroo’ production and e response latency to the first ‘kroo’ produced, in response to eagle, leopard and falling tree playbacks and depending on the alarm calling behaviour of associated male Campbell’s monkeys. Boxes show the inter quartile range, with the central line depicting the median, the whiskers indicating quantiles (0.025 and 0.975) and outliers

Full size image

Generally, we found that calling responses were only weakly affected by the type of danger, but strongly by the alarm calling behaviour of associated Campbell’s monkeys. In particular, the overall probability of calling (‘kroo’ or ‘tcha-kow’) was significantly affected by the interaction between the type of danger (i.e., eagle, leopard, falling tree) and Campbell’s monkey alarm calling (i.e., whether they called or not) (GLM1: ANOVA: Chi2 = 15.379, p < 0.001; Fig. 3a). Post hoc pairwise comparisons did not show a significant difference between the different combinations of contexts (see Online Resource 1, Table S5). The type of the first call produced was not affected by the type of danger but by whether or not there was prior Campbell’s monkey alarm calling (GLM2, ANOVA: Chi2 = 6.87, p = 0.009; Fig. 3b). In particular, ‘tcha-kows’ were produced only after Campbell’s monkey alarm calls.

Similarly, when analysing ‘kroo’ production efforts, we found no effect of the type of danger but a clear effect of Campbell’s monkey alarm calling on the number of ‘kroos’ produced, the rate of the three first ‘kroos’ and the response latency between the start of the stimulus and the first ‘kroo’ (N ‘kroo’: GLM3, ANOVA: Chi2 = 5.262, p = 0.022, Fig. 3c; initial ‘kroo’ rate: GLM4, ANOVA: Chi2 = 4.990, p = 0.026, Fig. 3d; ‘kroo’ response latency: GLM5, ANOVA: Chi2 = 18.498, p < 0.001; Fig. 3e).

Responses to Campbell’s monkey alarm calls

In the second experiment, we carried out N = 45 trials to investigate how male lesser spot-nosed monkeys used their calls in response to Campbell’s monkey alarm calls to eagles (‘hok’, N = 11), leopards (‘krak’, N = 14) and non-predatory dangers (‘boom’, N = 20). Again, we found that the type of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls did not affect the overall call response probability (GLMM1, ANOVA: Chi2 = 5.335, p = 0.069; Fig. 4a) although it did affect the type of first call produced (GLMM2, ANOVA: Chi2 = 12.894, p = 0.002; Fig. 4b). In particular, to Campbell’s monkey ‘booms’, male lesser spot-nosed monkeys produced mostly ‘tcha-kows’ whereas, to Campbell’s monkey ‘hoks’ or ‘kraks’, they uniformly responded with ‘kroos’ (pairwise post hoc comparisons ‘boom’ vs. ‘hok’: ß = 4.367, SE = 1.293, Z = 3.377, p = 0.002; ‘boom’ vs. ‘krak’: ß = 5.245, SE = 2.061, Z = 2.544, p = 0.028; ‘hok’ vs.’krak’; ß = − 0.878, SE = 1.910, Z = − 0.460, p = 0.887).

Fig. 4

Which of the following statements is correct regarding vervet monkey alarm calls?

Male lesser spot-nosed monkey a call production, b first call produced and c number of ‘kroo’ produced, in response to male Campbell’s monkey alarm calls playbacks (with associated contexts of production). Boxes show the inter quartile range, with the central line depicting the median, the whiskers indicating quantiles (0.025 and 0.975) and outliers. *p < 0.01

Full size image

The ‘kroo’ production effort to the three Campbell’s monkey alarm calls varied in number of ‘kroo’ produced (GLMM3, ANOVA: Chi2 = 7.837, p = 0.020; post hoc comparisons ‘hok’ vs. ‘boom’: ß = − 1.686, SE = 0.606, Z = − 2.783, p = 0.014; ‘krak’ vs. ‘boom’: ß = − 1.486, SE = 0.654, Z = − 2.274, p = 0.058; ‘hok’ vs. ‘krak’: ß = − 0.199, SE = 0.452, Z = − 0.441, p = 0.897; Fig. 4c) but not in the initial ‘kroo’ rate (GLMM4, ANOVA: Chi2 = 2.114, p = 0.146) nor in the ‘kroo’ response latency (GLMM5, ANOVA: Chi2 = 5.291, p = 0.071).

Finally, we observed that when lesser spot-nosed monkeys produced ‘tcha-kows’ in response to Campbell’s monkey ‘booms’, they consistently replied with a 10 s latency (mean = 10.0 s, SD = ± 1.06 s) following the first ‘boom’. In 54.5% of the trials (N = 11), males produced a second ‘tcha-kow’ (mean time interval between the two ‘tcha-kow’ = 0.27 s, SD = ± 0.23 s).

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the alarm call system of lesser spot-nosed monkeys and the influence of Campbell’s monkey vocal behaviour. Lesser spot-nosed and Campbell’s monkeys form robust mix-species associations, forage mainly in the lower canopy and have the same social organisation (McGraw and Rowe 1996; Buzzard 2006, 2010; McGraw et al. 2007; Daegling 2020). We first established the male lesser spot-nosed monkey call repertoire when encountering three types of danger (eagle, leopard and falling tree) and corresponding Campbell’s monkey alarm call, and identified three call types, ‘kroo’, ‘tcha’ and ‘kow’, the latter mainly produced as combinations (‘tcha-kow’; see Oates (1985) for alternative terminology). In playback experiments, we established that ‘kroos’ were given to any type of danger (eagles, leopards, falling trees) and also to male Campbell’s monkey predator-specific alarm calls (‘hok’, ‘krak’), but not to their calls produced in non-predatory contexts (‘boom’), suggesting that ‘kroos’ are equal to the ‘purring’ call reported by Rowe (1996).

We found no evidence for predator-specific differences in ‘kroo’ calling efforts, suggesting that ‘kroo’ production is unaffected by predator type. Equally surprising was the fact that we failed to find any evidence for acoustic variants within the ‘kroo’ call, neither in terms of predator-specificity nor in terms of individual identity. This was also found experimentally when we simulated danger with male Campbell’s monkey context-specific alarm calls. Again, male lesser spot-nosed monkeys responded uniformly by producing ‘kroos’, regardless of alarm call types, and ‘tcha-kows’ after Campbell’s monkey ‘booms’. This result is in contrast with most primate species that have been investigated so far in similar ways (e.g., Diana monkeys, Zuberbühler et al. 1997; Campbell’s monkeys, Zuberbühler 2001; tamarins, Kirchhof and Hammerschmidt 2006; vervet monkeys, Seyfarth et al. 1980; titi monkeys, Berthet et al. 2018; tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus paella), Wheeler 2010; blue monkeys, Mielke and Zuberbühler 2013; putty-nosed monkeys, Price et al. 2009; green monkeys (Chlorocebus sabaeus), Price and Fischer 2014) and requires explanation.

‘Kroo’ calls as general alarms

Lesser spot-nosed monkeys produce ‘kroo’ calls to a variety of dangers and thus qualify as general alarm or alert calls (Dezecache and Berthet 2018). General alarm calls have already been described for other primate and non-primate species, such as Gunnison’s prairie dogs (Cynomys gunnisoni) ‘bark’ calls for different predator types (Loughry et al. 2019) or woolly monkey (Lagothrix lagothricha) ‘eolk’ calls to predatory and non-predatory disturbances (Casamitjana 2002). Different functional explanations have been offered for general alarm calls (Dezecache and Berthet 2018), such as providing callers with the option to create a semantic contrast to predator-specific alarm calls, typically aerial alarm calls (e.g., dwarf mongoose (Helogale parvula), Collier et al. 2020; putty-nosed monkeys, Price et al. 2009), or to issue a behavioural imperative (e.g., ‘hide!’) to warn other group members or, in exceptional cases, deceptively, to gain access to food (Flower et al. 2014).

In our study, we did not find any evidence for predator-specific alarm calls. Instead, lesser spot-nosed monkeys used the same alarm call type, ‘kroo’, to any type of danger with only one further (combined) call, ‘tcha-kow’, given mainly in response to Campbell’s monkey calls produced in non-predatory contexts, ‘boom’ calls (Fig. 3a, b). In sum, lesser spot-nosed monkeys appear to have an alarm call system with a rather limited semantic capacity, not able to distinguish between predatory and non-predatory disturbances or event categories. Another possibility could be Dezecache and Berthet’s imperative hypothesis (2018), i.e., that ‘kroo’ calls function as instructions to other group members (e.g., ‘look around!’). In any case, more detailed behavioural observations are needed to understand the mechanisms of call production and receivers perception, ideally also from other study populations.

It is also possible that our acoustic analyses were simply not fine-scaled enough to pick up acoustic variation related to the event. If this was the case, our analyses demonstrate that acoustic variation in this species’ alarm calls would be considerably more subtle compared to what is normally seen in primate alarm calling (Fig. 2), especially regarding eagle alarms (Gautier and Gautier 1977). In order to address this hypothesis, it would be necessary to carry out playback experiments, to test whether recipients react differently to calls recorded to different predator types (e.g., Zuberbühler 2000). Furthermore, acoustic variations in alarm calls might not be associated with the type of danger but rather with other contextually relevant cues, such as predator behaviours (e.g., Griesser 2008), sizes (e.g., Templeton 2005), colours (Slobodchikoff et al. 2009), attack directions (e.g., Berthet et al. 2019) or response urgencies (e.g., Lemasson et al. 2010; Townsend et al. 2012; Murphy et al. 2013). Finally, and although we consider this also unlikely, future research should verify if any of the above information is encoded at the sequence level, similar to what has been described for Guereza colobus monkeys (Colobus guereza, Schel et al. 2010).

Eavesdropping and mixed-species associations between lesser spot-nosed and Campbell’s monkeys

A second major finding of this study was that lesser spot-nosed monkey alarm calling was heavily influenced by Campbell’s monkeys. For the ‘kroo’, the general pattern was that, if a male Campbell’s monkey called, the male lesser spot-nosed monkey reduced his own calling efforts compared to when Campbell’s monkeys did not call. The influence of male Campbell’s monkeys was even more striking when analysing ‘tcha-kow’ call production. Here, our results showed that this call type was produced, in almost all cases, about 10 s after the first Campbell’s monkey ‘boom’ call and hardly ever in other situations.

As mentioned, lesser spot-nosed monkeys spend most of their time in mixed-species associations with other primates, mostly Campbell’s monkeys. Such associations can provide predator avoidance and foraging advantages (Stensland et al. 2003) and vocal behaviour is a key mediator of any such benefits (Magrath et al. 2015). As proposed by Goodale et al. (2010), conspicuous vocal behaviour by one species can serve as a driving force in the formation or maintenance of mixed-species groups. For instance, in mixed-species groups of tamarins, the long calls of one species can cause vocal responses and approaches by the other (Windfelder 2001), suggesting that these calls could function to recruit hetero-specifics (Goodale et al. 2020).

Interspecies call recognition has mostly been studied with alarm calls (for review, see Magrath et al. 2015, 2020). The process is best referred to as ‘eavesdropping’, by which one species extracts information from another species’ vocal behaviour and then reacts appropriately, mostly in the context of predator avoidance and foraging (Goodale et al. 2010). Eavesdropping is widespread with a large body of evidence in primates (Zuberbühler et al. 1997; Ramakrishnan and Coss 2000; Fichtel 2008; Walton and Kershenbaum 2019) but also in birds ( Fallow and Magrath 2010; Dawson Pell et al. 2018; Keen et al. 2020) and even reptiles. For example, non-vocal iguanas (Amblyrhynchus cristatus and Oplurus cuvieri) can discriminate mockingbird (Nesomimus parvulus) and paradise flycatcher (Terpsiphone mutate) alarm calls (Ito and Mori 2010; Vitousek et al. 2007). However, in many species, eavesdropping individuals respond with their own alarm calls to the calls of another species (Shriner 1998; Ramakrishnan and Coss 2000; Zuberbühler 2000; Rainey et al. 2004; Slater et al. 2004).

In our study, results suggest that lesser spot-nosed monkeys discriminated Campbell’s monkey alarm calls, as males responded with ‘kroos’ to male Campbell’s monkey eagle and leopard alarm calls and ‘tcha-kows’ to Campbell’s monkey ‘booms’. However, whether they also discriminated Campbell’s monkey eagle (‘hok’) and leopard alarms (‘krak’) as predator-specific alarm calls cannot be decided by our data. Nevertheless, we found that lesser spot-nosed monkey calling efforts decreased if Campbell’s monkeys alarm called compared to when they did not call. Overall, these calling patterns are compatible with the hypotheses that lesser spot-nosed monkeys benefit from an associated species to minimize their own exposure to predators by eavesdropping on their alarm calls.

The large majority of ‘tcha-kows’ were produced immediately after Campbell’s monkey ‘boom’ calls, and only rarely after situations that elicited the ‘boom’ calls (i.e., mostly falling trees), suggesting that the calls did not primarily function to refer to a type of danger. Instead, we see two possible explanations. First, ‘tcha-kows’ were produced to secure associations with Campbell’s monkeys. Both species form strong, semi-permanent associations (Buzzard 2010) and male calls function to mediate such relationships. Second, ‘tcha-kows’ were used as territorial defence to warn rival males and produced to benefit from the attention raised by the preceding Campbell’s monkey ‘booms’, which travel over considerable distances (Zuberbühler 2004; McGraw et al. 2007). Moreover, if predators are attracted by such male loud calls, then male lesser spot-nosed monkeys can reduce the number of such events, by aligning their own calls with the calling of associated Campbell’s monkeys, rather than calling independently. Further studies focusing on receiver and predator behaviour is needed to learn how they react to ‘tcha-kow’ calls with or without preceding Campbell’s monkey ‘boom’ calls.

Limitations of playback experiments

Our study was based on a well-tried paradigm, simulating predator presence with acoustic cues. This approach is especially effective for species living in dense forest habitats, where individuals rely heavily on auditory information to learn about ongoing events in their vicinity. Although we have managed to collect samples from a large 50km2 area of Taï National Park, our final sample size for each of the various conditions was relatively small, due to the difficulty of obtaining high quality and statistically independent recordings, a clear limitation to this study. In addition, playbacks are an incomplete way of investigating the mechanisms and function of animal vocal behaviour, unless they are complemented with careful, long-term behavioural observations. Also, simulating predator presence by acoustic means is suboptimal because the critical predator information is provided to everyone at the same time, which arguably makes warning others obsolete. Complementary experiments with realistic visual predator models would be needed to see if patterns change when predator information is administered individually.

Conclusions

We have analysed the alarm call behaviour of male lesser spot-nosed monkeys, a West African forest guenon exposed to high predation pressure from different predators. Contrary to the alarm systems of other Cercopithecus monkeys living in similar environments, we found no evidence for predator-specific alarm calls in this species. Instead, we found evidence for a general alarm call, ‘kroo’, given to all types of dangers (eagles, leopards and falling trees) and a more specialized call, ‘tcha-kow’, given presumably to form mix-group associations with Campbell’s monkeys and possibly to repel sexual rivals. For both call types, production was heavily influenced by the calling behaviour of associated Campbell’s monkeys, with a reduction of the ‘kroo’ calling effort when associated male Campbell’s monkeys produced their own alarm calls and a temporarily tight, duet-like production of ‘tcha-kows’ in response to Campbell’s monkey ‘boom’ calls. We concluded that male lesser spot-nosed monkeys adjust their call production presumably to reduce their own exposure to predators by eavesdropping on Campbell’s monkeys and to secure mixed-species association.

References

  • Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2006) The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Anim Behav 72:643–653. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.11.017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baker MC, Becker AM (2002) Mobbing calls of black-capped chickadees: effects of urgency on call production. Wilson Bull 114:510–516

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthet M, Mesbahi G, Pajot A, Cäsar C, Neumann C, Zuberbühler K (2019) Titi monkeys combine alarm calls to create probabilistic meaning. Sci Adv 5:eaav3991. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aav3991

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Berthet M, Neumann C, Mesbahi G, Cäsar C, Zuberbühler K (2018) Contextual encoding in titi monkey alarm call sequences. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 72:8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-017-2424-z

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C (1994) Cooperative hunting in wild chimpanzees. Anim Behav 48:653–667. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1994.1285

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boesch C, Boesch H (1989) Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Am J Phys Anthropol 78:547–573. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.1330780410

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buzzard PJ (2010) Polyspecific associations of Cercopithecus campbelli and C. petaurista with C. diana: what are the costs and benefits? Primates 51:307–314. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-010-0203-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buzzard PJ (2006) Ecological partitioning of Cercopithecus campbelli, C. petaurista, and C. diana in the Taï Forest. Int J Primatol 27:529–558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-006-9022-7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A (2012) Convergence and divergence in Diana monkey vocalizations. Biol Lett 8:382–385. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.1182

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Caro TM (1986) The functions of stotting: a review of the hypotheses. Anim Behav 34:649–662. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80051-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casamitjana J (2002) The vocal repertoire of the woolly monkey Lagothrix lagothricha. Bioacoustics 13:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2002.9753483

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cäsar C, Byrne RW, Hoppitt W, Young RJ, Zuberbühler K (2012) Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls. Anim Behav 84:405–411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman CA, Chapman LJ (1996) Mixed-species primate groups in the kibale forest: ecological constraints on association. Int J Primatol 17:31–50. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02696157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman CA, Chapman LJ (2000) Interdemic variation in mixed-species association patterns: common diurnal primates of Kibale National Park, Uganda. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 47:129–139. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050003

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung Y, Rabe-Hesketh S, Dorie V, Gelman A, Liu J (2013) A nondegenerate penalized likelihood estimator for variance parameters in multilevel models. Psychometrika 78:685–709. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-013-9328-2

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collier K, Radford AN, Stoll S, Watson SK, Manser MB, Bickel B, Townsend SW (2020) Dwarf mongoose alarm calls: investigating a complex non-human animal call. Proc R Soc B 287:20192514. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2514

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cords M (1990) Vigilance and mixed-species association of some East African forest monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 26:297–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00178323

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Covey R, McGraw WS (2014) Monkeys in a West African bushmeat market: implications for Cercopithecid conservation in Eastern Liberia. Trop Conserv Sci 7:115–125. https://doi.org/10.1177/194008291400700103

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coye C, Ouattara K, Zuberbühler K, Lemasson A (2015) Suffixation influences receivers’ behaviour in non-human primates. Proc R Soc B 282:20150265. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0265

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham S, Magrath RD (2017) Functionally referential alarm calls in noisy miners communicate about predator behaviour. Anim Behav 129:171–179. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson Pell FSE, Potvin DA, Ratnayake CP, Fernández-Juricic E, Magrath RD, Radford AN (2018) Birds orient their heads appropriately in response to functionally referential alarm calls of heterospecifics. Anim Behav 140:109–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.04.010

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dezecache G, Berthet M (2018) Working hypotheses on the meaning of general alarm calls. Anim Behav 142:113–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.06.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eckardt W, Zuberbühler K (2004) Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behav Ecol 15:400–411. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh032

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engesser S, Ridley AR, Townsend SW (2016) Meaningful call combinations and compositional processing in the southern pied babbler. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113:5976–5981. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1600970113

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fallow PM, Magrath RD (2010) Eavesdropping on other species: mutual interspecific understanding of urgency information in avian alarm calls. Anim Behav 79:411–417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.11.018

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C (2008) Ontogeny of conspecific and heterospecific alarm call recognition in wild Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi verreauxi). Am J Primatol 70:127–135. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20464

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, Kappeler PM (2002) Anti-predator behavior of group-living Malagasy primates: mixed evidence for a referential alarm call system. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 51:262–275. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-001-0436-0

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtel C, van Schaik CP (2006) Semantic differences in Sifaka (Propithecus verreauxi) alarm calls: a reflection of genetic or cultural variants? Ethology 112:839–849. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01239.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer J, Hammerschmidt K, Todt D (2010) Factors affecting acoustic variation in Barbary macaque (Macaca sylvanus) disturbance calls. Ethology 101:51–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1995.tb00345.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer J, Metz M, Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2001) Baboon responses to graded bark variants. Anim Behav 61:925–931. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1687

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flower TP, Gribble M, Ridley AR (2014) Deception by flexible alarm mimicry in an African bird. Science 344:513. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1249723

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foerster S (2008) Two incidents of venomous snakebite on juvenile blue and Sykes monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis stuhlmanni and C. m. albogularis). Primates 49:300–303. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-008-0098-x

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox J, Weisberg S (2019) An {R} Companion to Applied Regression, version 3.0–10, https://socialsciences.mcmaster.ca/jfox/Books/Companion/

  • Gamer M, Lemon J, Ian Fellows Puspendra Singh (2019) irr: Various Coefficients of Interrater Reliability and Agreement, R package version 0.84.1, https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=irr

  • Gautier J-P, Gautier A (1977) Communication in old world monkeys. In: University I (ed) How animals communicate Sebeok TA. Press, Bloomington, pp 890–964

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Beauchamp G, Magrath RD, Nieh JC, Ruxton GD (2010) Interspecific information transfer influences animal community structure. Trends Ecol Evol 25:354–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2010.01.002

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Ruxton GD, Beauchamp G (2019) Predator eavesdropping in a mixed-species environment: how prey species may use grouping, confusion, and the cocktail party effect to reduce predator detection. Front Ecol Evol 7:141. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2019.00141

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodale E, Sridhar H, Sieving KE et al (2020) Mixed company: a framework for understanding the composition and organization of mixed-species animal groups. Biol Rev 95:889–910. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12591

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Graw B, Manser MB (2007) The function of mobbing in cooperative meerkats. Anim Behav 74:507–517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.11.021

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2008) Referential calls signal predator behavior in a group-living bird species. Curr Biol 18:69–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.069

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griesser M (2009) Mobbing calls signal predator category in a kin group-living bird species. Proc R Soc Lond B 276:2887–2892. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.0551

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grieves LA, Logue DM, Quinn JS (2014) Joint-nesting smooth-billed anis, Crotophaga ani, use a functionally referential alarm call system. Anim Behav 89:215–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.01.008

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heymann EW (2011) Coordination in primate mixed-species groups. In: Boos M, Kolbe M, Kappeler PM, Ellwart T (eds) Coordination in Human and Primate Groups. Springer, Heidelberg, pp 263–281

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hothorn T, Bretz F, Westfall P (2008) Simultaneous inference in general parametric. Biometrical J 50:346–363

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ito R, Mori A (2010) Vigilance against predators induced by eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls in a non-vocal lizard Oplurus cuvieri cuvieri (Reptilia: Iguania). Proc R Soc Lond B 277:1275–1280. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.2047

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenny D (2005) Hunting behaviour in West African forest leopards. Afr J Ecol 43:197–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2028.2005.00565.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kane EE, McGraw WS (2017) Dietary variation in Diana monkeys (Cercopithecus diana): the effects of polyspecific associations. Folia Primatol 88:455–482. https://doi.org/10.1159/000484560

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keen SC, Cole EF, Sheehan MJ, Sheldon BC (2020) Social learning of acoustic anti-predator cues occurs between wild bird species. Proc R Soc B 287:20192513. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2513

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan S, Mathevon N, Stevens JM, Nicolè F, Zuberbühler K, Guéry JP, Levréro F (2020) The reliability of individual vocal signature varies across the bonobo’s graded repertoire. Anim Behav 169:9–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2020.08.024

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kershenbaum A, Blumstein DT, Roch MA et al (2016) Acoustic sequences in non-human animals: a tutorial review and prospectus. Biol Rev 91:13–52. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12160

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchhof J, Hammerschmidt K (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax) - evidence from playback experiments. Ethology 112:346–354. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01165.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klump GM, Kretzschmar E, Curio E (1986) The hearing of an avian predator and its avian prey. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 18:317–323. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299662

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koné I, McGraw S, Gonedelé Bi S, Oates JF (2019) Cercopithecus diana: The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2019: e.T4245A92384250, https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2019-3.RLTS.T4245A92384250.en.

  • Lê S, Josse J, Husson F (2008) FactoMineR: an R package for multivariate analysis. J Stat Softw 25:1–18. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v025.i01

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Bouchet H, Zuberbühler K (2010) Speed of call delivery is related to context and caller identity in Campbell’s monkey males. Naturwissenschaften 97:1023–1027. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-010-0715-6

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loughry WJ, Oeser M, Anderson CD, Hoogland JL (2019) The importance of individual variation in the alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs. Anim Behav 150:59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.01.019

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Haff TM, Fallow PM, Radford AN (2015) Eavesdropping on heterospecific alarm calls: from mechanisms to consequences: interspecific eavesdropping. Biol Rev 90:560–586. https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12122

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Magrath RD, Haff TM, Igic B (2020) Interspecific communication: gaining information from heterospecific alarm calls. In: Aubin T, Mathevon N (eds) Coding Strategies in Vertebrate Acoustic Communication. Springer International Publishing, Cham, pp 287–314

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Manser MB (2001) The acoustic structure of suricates’ alarm calls varies with predator type and the level of response urgency. Proc R Soc Lond B 268:2315–2324

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Matsuda Goodwin R, Gonedelé Bi S, Koné I (2019) Cercopithecus campbelli: The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T136930A92374066, https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-2.RLTS.T136930A92374066.en

  • Matsuda Goodwin R, Segniagbeto G, Wiafe E, Osei D, Koné I, Gonedelé Bi S, Oates JF (2017) Cercopithecus petaurista. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T4225A17945536. International Union for Conservation of Nature, https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-2.RLTS.T4225A17945536.en

  • McGraw S, Minhós T, Bersacola E, Ferreira da Silva MJ, Galat-Luong A, Gonedelé Bi S, Mayhew M, Oates JF, Starin ED (2020) Piliocolobus badius: The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2020: e.T161247840A161259430. https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2020-1.RLTS.T161247840A161259430.en

  • McGraw WS (2000) Positional behavior of Cercopithecus petaurista. Int J Primatol 21:157–182. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005483815514

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw WS, Daegling DJ (2020) Diet, feeding behavior, and jaw architecture of Taï monkeys: Congruence and chaos in the realm of functional morphology. Evol Anthropol 29:14–28. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21799

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw WS, Zuberbühler K (2008) Socioecology, predation, and cognition in a community of West African monkeys. Evol Anthropol 17:254–266. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGraw WS, Zuberbühler K, Noë R (2007) Monkeys of the Taï forest: an African primate community. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mielke A, Zuberbühler K (2013) A method for automated individual, species and call type recognition in free-ranging animals. Anim Behav 86:475–482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.04.017

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani M (1991) Niche overlap and polyspecific associations among sympatric cercopithecids in the campo animal reserve, Southwestern Cameroon. Primates 32:137–151. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381172

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mourthé ÍMC, Guedes D, Fidelis J, Boubli JP, Mendes SL, Strier KB (2007) Ground use by northern muriquis (Brachyteles hypoxanthus). Am J Primatol 69:706–712. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20405

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mundry R, Sommer C (2007) Discriminant function analysis with nonindependent data: consequences and an alternative. Anim Behav 74:965–976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.12.028

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy D, Lea SEG, Zuberbühler K (2013) Male blue monkey alarm calls encode predator type and distance. Anim Behav 85:119–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.10.015

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oates JF (1985) The Nigerian Guenon, Cercopithecus erythrogaster: ecological, behavioral, systematic and historical observations. Folia Primatol 45:25–43. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156189

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K (2009a) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106:22026–22031. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908118106

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K (2009b) Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS ONE 4:e7808. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007808

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Penner J, Fruteau C, Range F, Rödel M-O (2008) Finding a needle in a haystack: new methods of locating and working with rhinoceros vipers (Bitis rhinoceros). Herpetol Rev 39:310–314

    Google Scholar 

  • Price T, Arnold K, Zuberbühler K, Semple S (2009) Pyow but not hack calls of the male putty-nosed monkey (Cercopithcus nictitans) convey information about caller identity. Behaviour 146:871–888. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853908X396610

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price T, Fischer J (2014) Meaning attribution in the West African green monkey: influence of call type and context. Anim Cogn 17:277–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0660-9

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Price T, Wadewitz P, Cheney D, Seyfarth R, Hammerschmidt K, Fischer J (2015) Vervets revisited: a quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity. Sci Rep 5:13220. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13220

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • R Core Team (2020) R: A language and environment for statistical computing, version 1.3.1093. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria, https://www.R-project.org/

  • Rainey HJ, Zuberbühler K, Slater PJB (2004) Hornbills can distinguish between primate alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond B 271:755–759. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2619

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramakrishnan U, Coss RG (2000) Recognition of heterospecific alarm vocalization by Bonnet Macaques (Macaca radiata). J Comp Psychol 114:3–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.3

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Range F, Fischer J (2004) Vocal repertoire of Sooty mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus atys) in the Tai National Park. Ethology 110:301–321. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.00973.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rauber R (2020) Call order within vocal sequences of meerkats contains temporary contextual and individual information. BMC Biol 18:119

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowe N (1996) The pictorial guide to the living primates. Pogonias Press, East Hampton

    Google Scholar 

  • Schel AM, Candiotti A, Zuberbühler K (2010) Predator-deterring alarm call sequences in Guereza colobus monkeys are meaningful to conspecifics. Anim Behav 80:799–808. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.07.012

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schlenker P, Chemla E, Arnold K, Zuberbühler K (2016) Pyow-hack revisited: two analyses of Putty-nosed monkey alarm calls. Lingua 171:1–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.10.002

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL, Marler P (1980) Vervet monkey alarm calls: semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim Behav 28:1070–1094. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalter M (1978) Localization of passerine seeet and mobbing calls by goshawks and pygmy owls. Z Tierpsychol 46:260–267. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1978.tb01448.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman PW (1977) Nepotism and the evolution of alarm calls. Science 197:1246–1253. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.197.4310.1246

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shriner WM (1998) Yellow-bellied marmot and golden-mantled ground squirrel responses to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim Behav 55:529–536. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0623

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shultz S (2008) Population density, breeding chronology and diet of Crowned Eagles Stephanoaetus coronatus in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Ibis 144:135–138. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0019-1019.2001.00005.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slater P, Rainey H, Zuberbühler K (2004) The responses of black-casqued hornbills to predator vocalisations and primate alarm calls. Behaviour 141:1263–1277. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539042729658

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobodchikoff CN, Paseka A, Verdolin JL (2009) Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors. Anim Cogn 12:435–439. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-008-0203-y

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stensland E, Angerbjörn A, Berggren P (2003) Mixed species groups in mammals. Mammal Rev 33:205–223

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan C, Zuberbühler K (2016) Persistent females and compliant males coordinate alarm calling in Diana monkeys. Curr Biol 26:2907–2912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.08.033

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Struhsaker TT (1967) Auditory communication among vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aetthiops). In: Altmann SA (ed) Social communication among primates. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp 281–324

    Google Scholar 

  • Struhsaker TT (1981) Polyspecific associations among tropical rain-forest primates. Z Tierpsychol 57:268–304. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1981.tb01928.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2012) Referential mobbing calls elicit different predator-searching behaviours in Japanese great tits. Anim Behav 84:53–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.03.030

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2014) Communication about predator type by a bird using discrete, graded and combinatorial variation in alarm calls. Anim Behav 87:59–65

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki TN (2016) Referential calls coordinate multi-species mobbing in a forest bird community. J Ethol 34:79–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10164-015-0449-1

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton CN (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science 308:1934–1937. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108841

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend SW, Rasmussen M, Clutton-Brock T, Manser MB (2012) Flexible alarm calling in meerkats: the role of the social environment and predation urgency. Behav Ecol 23:1360–1364. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/ars129

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venables WN, Ripley BD (2002) Modern Applied Statistics with S, 4th edn. Springer, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vitousek MN, Adelman JS, Gregory NC, Clair JJHS (2007) Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile. Biol Lett 3:632–634. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0443

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Vooren AP (1986) Nature and origin of tree and branch fall in the Taï Forest (Ivory Coast). Neth J Agric Sci 34:112–116. https://doi.org/10.18174/njas.v34i1.16823

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton B, Kershenbaum A (2019) Heterospecific recognition of referential alarm calls in two species of lemur. Bioacoustics 28:592–603. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2018.1509375

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler BC (2010) Production and perception of situationally variable alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64:989–1000. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0914-3

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wich SA, de Vries H (2006) Male monkeys remember which group members have given alarm calls. Proc R Soc Lond B 273:735–740. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2005.3320

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Windfelder TL (2001) Interspecific communication in mixed-species groups of tamarins: evidence from playback experiments. Anim Behav 61:1193–1201. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1701

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolters S, Zuberbühler K (2003) Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell’s monkeys: the costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. Behaviour 140:371–385. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853903321826684

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2000) Interspecies semantic communication in two forest primates. Proc R Soc Lond B 267:713–718. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2000.1061

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2001) Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell’s monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 50:414–422. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650100383

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2002) A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Anim Behav 63:293–299. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1914

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K (2004) Effects of natural and sexual selection on the evolution of guenon loud calls. In: Glenn ME, Cords M (eds) The Guenons: Diversity and Adaptation in African Monkeys. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Boston, pp 289–306

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Jenny D (2002) Leopard predation and primate evolution. J Hum Evol 43:873–886. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.2002.0605

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Jenny D, Bshary R (1999) The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology 105:477–490. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00396.x

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zuberbühler K, Noë R, Seyfarth RM (1997) Diana monkey long-distance calls: messages for conspecifics and predators. Anim Behav 53:589–604. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0334

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Which of the following statements are true about tarsiers?

Which of the following is true about tarsiers? Correct Answer(s) : They have large, bulbous eyes.

Which of the following are central features regarding chimpanzee tool use?

Which of the following are central features regarding chimpanzee tool use? Correct Answer(s): Chimpanzees are intelligent enough to engage in behaviors that require learning.

What are the three major factors that impact a primates feeding strategy?

(Q002) Quality, distribution, and availability are the three key factors that contribute to primates' success at feeding. Which of the following is true of such success? Primates adjust the size of the feeding group based on how much food is in a patch.

Which of the following is true about the human primate interface for macaques in southeast asia?

Which of the following is true about the human-primate interface for macaques in southeast Asia? Correct Answer(s): - Farmers often tolerate macaques stealing crops because of local folklore.