People who belong to the same social class are most likely to have which of the following in common?

If you're seeing this message, it means we're having trouble loading external resources on our website.

If you're behind a web filter, please make sure that the domains *.kastatic.org and *.kasandbox.org are unblocked.

Social class refers to a group of people with similar levels of wealth, influence, and status. Sociologists typically use three methods to determine social class:

  • The objective method measures and analyzes “hard” facts.
  • The subjective method asks people what they think of themselves.
  • The reputational method asks what people think of others.

Results from these three research methods suggests that in the United States today approximately 15 to 20 percent are in the poor, lower class; 30 to 40 percent are in the working class; 40 to 50 percent are in the middle class; and 1 to 3 percent are in the rich, upper class.

The lower class is typified by poverty, homelessness, and unemployment. People of this class, few of whom have finished high school, suffer from lack of medical care, adequate housing and food, decent clothing, safety, and vocational training. The media often stigmatize the lower class as “the underclass,” inaccurately characterizing poor people as welfare mothers who abuse the system by having more and more babies, welfare fathers who are able to work but do not, drug abusers, criminals, and societal “trash.” The working class are those minimally educated people who engage in “manual labor” with little or no prestige. Unskilled workers in the class—dishwashers, cashiers, maids, and waitresses—usually are underpaid and have no opportunity for career advancement. They are often called the working poor. Skilled workers in this class—carpenters, plumbers, and electricians—are often called blue collar workers. They may make more money than workers in the middle class—secretaries, teachers, and computer technicians; however, their jobs are usually more physically taxing, and in some cases quite dangerous. The middle class are the “sandwich” class. These white collar workers have more money than those below them on the “social ladder,” but less than those above them. They divide into two levels according to wealth, education, and prestige. The lower middle class is often made up of less educated people with lower incomes, such as managers, small business owners, teachers, and secretaries. The upper middle class is often made up of highly educated business and professional people with high incomes, such as doctors, lawyers, stockbrokers, and CEOs. Comprising only 1 to 3 percent of the United States population, the upper class holds more than 25 percent of the nation's wealth. This class divides into two groups: lower‐upper and upper‐upper. The lower‐upper class includes those with “new money,” or money made from investments, business ventures, and so forth. The upper‐upper class includes those aristocratic and “high‐society” families with “old money” who have been rich for generations. These extremely wealthy people live off the income from their inherited riches. The upper‐upper class is more prestigious than the lower‐upper class.

Wherever their money comes from, both segments of the upper class are exceptionally rich. Both groups have more money than they could possibly spend, which leaves them with much leisure time for cultivating a variety of interests. They live in exclusive neighborhoods, gather at expensive social clubs, and send their children to the finest schools. As might be expected, they also exercise a great deal of influence and power both nationally and globally.

  • Journal List
  • BMJ
  • v.323(7326); 2001 Dec 15
  • PMC60984

BMJ. 2001 Dec 15; 323(7326): 1398–1401.

Fiona Mulvany, researcher,a Eadbhard O'Callaghan, professor,b Noriyoshi Takei, associate professor,c Majella Byrne, researcher,a Paul Fearon, psychiatrist,d and Conall Larkin, psychiatrista

Abstract

Objectives

To examine if low parental social class increases children's risk of subsequently developing schizophrenia or modifies the presentation.

Design

Case-control study with historical controls.

Setting

Geographically defined region in south Dublin.

Participants

352 patients with first presentation of schizophrenia matched with the next registered same sex birth from the same birth registration district.

Main outcome measures

Social class at birth. Age at presentation to psychiatric services, admission to hospital, and diagnosis of schizophrenia.

Results

Risk of schizophrenia was not increased in people from lower social classes. There was a slight excess risk among people in highest social classes (odds ratio 0.59, 95% confidence interval 0.40 to 0.85). However, the mean age at presentation was 24.8 years for patients whose parents were in the highest social class compared with 33.1 years for those in the lowest social class at birth.

Conclusions

Although social class of origin does not seem to be an important risk factor for schizophrenia, it partially determines the age at which patients receive treatment. The relation between low social class at birth and poor outcome may be at least partially mediated through treatment delay.

What is already known on this topic

Schizophrenia is more common in people from lower social classes

This could be due to increased vulnerability or social drift

What this study adds

Low social class at birth was not associated with increased risk of schizophrenia

People from lower social classes were older at first contact with psychiatric services than those from higher social classes

Delay in treatment of psychosis may explain the adverse outcome among people in low social classes

Introduction

Psychiatric disorders have been consistently shown to be more common among people in lower social classes.1,2 Schizophrenia is the mental illness most strongly linked to class, with working class people being about five times more likely to be diagnosed with schizophrenia than other groups.3

Many people who develop schizophrenia do not achieve or maintain the social class they were born into. By the time they have contact with psychiatric services, patients have often moved into a lower social class.4,5 However, this does not exclude the possibility that low social class increases the risk of later schizophrenia.6,7 Evidence is accumulating that the origins of the disorder lie in early life, and various environmental factors have been shown to be associated with an increased risk of later schizophrenia.8 These factors include obstetric complications,9,10 prenatal infections,11 and nutritional deprivation,12 all of which are more common among people in lower social classes.13–15 However, it remains unclear whether people born into lower social classes are at increased risk of schizophrenia.1,16,17

Social class at birth could also modify the course of the disorder. There is often a considerable delay between the onset of psychotic symptoms and adequate treatment. People who have a longer duration of untreated psychosis have been shown to have a poorer outcome,18,19 and social class influences the age at which people with other illnesses present to healthcare services.20 Using a case-control design, we investigated whether social class of origin influenced the risk of schizophrenia and the age at which patients first presented to psychiatric services for treatment.

Participants and methods

The sample was drawn from patients referred to Cluain Mhuire Family Centre, a geographically defined community based psychiatric service for a population of 165 000 people. Cases were limited to patients who were subsequently admitted to Saint John of God Hospital for the first time with schizophrenia (diagnosis based on ICD-9, international classification of diseases, 9th edition). We selected a consecutive case series of patients (n=629) discharged from their first admission to hospital between January 1984 and May 1993. We compiled clinical and demographic data for each patient within the catchment area from case notes and computerised records. We defined age at first contact with psychiatric service as any inpatient or outpatient contact with a mental health professional. The age at first diagnosis was defined as the date at which schizophrenia was diagnosed.

We searched the birth registration records of the 629 patients with the help of the General Register Office and recorded details of their father's occupation at the time of their birth. We were unable to locate the records for 140 patients, and a further 64 patients were excluded because they were not born in Ireland (19 people), they had an incorrect date of birth (4), details of fathers' occupation were not recorded (2), or their maiden name was missing (39 married women).

We recorded the paternal occupation of the next registered same sex birth from the birth registration district of the patient and used that child as a matched control. Paternal occupational data were compiled in accordance with the Census of Population Classification of Occupations, which consists of six categories (box).21 Farmers are assigned to a social class category on the basis of farm acreage, but this was not included on the birth registration record. We were therefore unable to assign social class categories to 31 cases and 51 controls. We were also unable to assign social class codes to fathers described on the birth certificate as unemployed (2 cases, 2 controls), student (1 case, 1 control), or pensioner (1 control). The final sample consisted of 391 cases and 370 controls, of which 352 were matched pairs.

Social class scale21

Social class I—Higher professional, higher managerial, proprietors, and farmers with ⩾200 acres (81 hectares)

Social class II—Lower professional, lower managerial, and farmers with 100-199 acres

Social class III—Other non-manual and farmers with 50-99 acres

Social class IV—Skilled manual and farmers with 30-49 acres

Social class V—Semiskilled manual and farmers with <30 acres

Social class VI—Unskilled manual

Power

With 352 case-control pairs, this study had a power of 87% to detect a difference of 10% in the proportions of discordant pairs (classes I-III v IV-VI) with respect to social class of origin provided that the proportion of discordant pairs was 37% at a two sided 5% significance level. We had sufficient patients in social classes I and VI to detect a difference between the two classes of 8.5 years in the age of first contact (80% power), 8.2 years in the age of first ever admission (78% power), and 10 years in the age of first admission with schizophrenia (88% power) at a two sided 1% significance level.

Statistical analysis

We investigated the distribution of social class backgrounds between cases and controls using the Wilcoxon matched pairs signed rank test. We then examined the proportions of discordant pairs for social class using McNemar's test, in which a binary social class variable (social class I-III=high, social class IV-VI=low) was created. In addition, we used a model of conditional logistic regression to fit the six social class categories as an explanatory variable. Next, we carried out analysis of variance to examine any difference in the age at first contact with psychiatric services, age at first ever admission, and age at first admission with a diagnosis of schizophrenia among the six social class groups. Finally, we fitted a regression model to quantify differences in ages at first use of the psychiatric services among the six social class of groups. Except for the conditional logistic regression analysis (STATA programme), we used the SPSS statistical package throughout.

Results

Social class as risk factor for schizophrenia

There was no overall significant difference between cases and controls in the distribution of social class (Wilcoxon rank sum z=−1.3912, P=0.16). Within the 352 matched pairs, the patients and controls came from the same social class in 77 pairs; patients were from a higher social class in 149 pairs and from a lower social class in 126 pairs. The odds ratio of developing the disorder associated with social class (low v high) was 0.59 (95% confidence interval 0.40 to 0.85, P<0.0029), indicating that people from low social classes have a reduced risk of schizophrenia compared with those from high social classes.

Table 1 shows that the risk of developing schizophrenia varies across the six social classes of origin. There was no consistent trend in odds ratios across the social classes, although people in lower classes tended to have a lower risk than those in higher classes. However, the likelihood ratio statistic from conditional logistic regression was 11.39 (df=5, P=0.044), indicating a significant overall difference in risk across social classes.

Table 1

Risk of developing schizophrenia according to social class at birth

Social classOdds ratio95% CI
I 1
II 0.93 0.52 to 1.68
III 0.99 0.66 to 1.75
IV 0.50 0.28 to 0.90*
V 0.52 0.27 to 1.02
VI 0.70 0.38 to 1.31

Relation between social class and age at first presentation

There was an overall significant difference in the age at first contact among the six social classes (F=2.95, df=5, 345; P=0.01). The mean age at first contact with psychiatric services was 30.1 years in the whole sample (men 28.6 years; women 32.2 years). Patients in social class I were youngest at first contact (25.3 years) and those in social class VI the oldest (34.4 years). Similarly, there was an overall significant difference in the age at first ever admission (F=2.55, df=5, 345; P=0.03). The mean age at first ever admission to a psychiatric hospital was 29.6 years (men 28.2 years; women 31.6 years). Patients in social class I were youngest at first ever admission (24.8 years) and those in social class VI oldest (33.1 years). We also found a significant difference in the age at first admission with schizophrenia (F=3.92, df=5, 345; P=0.002); the mean age at first admission with schizophrenia was 33.3 years (men 31.4 years; women 36.0 years), with patients in social class I being the youngest (28.0 years) and those in social class VI the oldest (38.8 years).

We found no evidence that sex modified the effect in any of the above measures (F=1.22, df=5, 340; P=0.30 for age at first contact; F=1.51, df=5, 340; P=0.19 for age at first admission; and F=1.84, df=5, 340; P=0.10 for age at first admission with schizophrenia), which indicates that age differences between the two sexes are identical across social classes. Sex distribution did not differ significantly across the social classes (χ2=7.50, df=5, P=0.19). Thus, sex is unlikely to confound the results. Nevertheless, we controlled for sex in the estimates of differences in age of contact, admission, and diagnosis between patients from different social classes.

Table 2 shows that men tended to be younger than women from the same social class background at time of first contact with psychiatric services, first admission, and diagnosis of schizophrenia. Analysis of variance showed a significant main effect of sex for age at first contact (F=5.87, df=1, 345; P=0.016), age at first ever admission (F=5.41, df=1, 345; P=0.021), and age at first admission with schizophrenia (F=9.30, df=1, 345; P=0.002).

Table 2

Age at first contact with psychiatric services, first admission, and first diagnosis of schizophrenia by social class

Social classNoMean (SD) age at first contact (years)Mean(SD) age at first admission (years)Mean (SD) age at first admission with schizophrenia (years)
All 352 30.1 (13.1) 29.6 (12.8) 33.3 (13.9)
 Men 205 28.6 (12.0) 28.2 (11.6) 31.4 (12.2)
 Women 147 32.2 (14.4) 31.6 (14.0) 36.0 (15.6)
I 48 25.3 (9.1) 24.8 (8.2) 28.0 (8.7)
 Men 31 24.4 (6.1) 24.0 (6.0) 26.6 (6.0)
 Women 17 27.1 (13.0) 26.4 (11.3) 30.4 (11.9)
II 60 31.1 (12.7) 31.0 (12.3) 34.1 (13.6)
 Men 26 30.3 (11.4) 30.2 (11.3) 32.0 (11.9)
 Women 34 31.7 (13.7) 31.7 (13.2) 35.7 (14.8)
III 68 29.8 (13.9) 29.3 (13.1) 33.0 (14.3)
 Men 44 29.8 (14.0) 29.4 (13.7) 32.8 (14.0)
 Women 24 29.9 (14.1) 29.0 (12.3) 33.4 (15.1)
IV 68 29.1 (12.3) 29.2 (12.5) 32.0 (13.3)
 Men 40 27.7 (10.3) 28.0 (10.6) 30.7 (10.5)
 Women 28 31.0 (14.8) 30.9 (14.9) 33.9 (16.5)
V 39 28.8 (14.0) 28.4 (13.6) 31.9 (14.6)
 Men 23 28.3 (16.2) 28.2 (15.5) 31.8 (16.5)
 Women 16 29.6 (10.5) 28.6 (10.8) 32.1 (11.9)
VI 69 34.4 (14.4) 33.1 (14.4) 38.8 (15.3)
 Men 41 30.3 (12.0) 28.9 (11.1) 33.6 (12.4)
 Women 28 40.4 (15.6) 39.3 (16.5) 46.5 (16.0)

Discussion

We found no link between social class at birth and risk of schizophrenia. However, people in lower social classes tended to present later than those in higher social classes. Men also presented at a younger age than women in the same social class.

Validity of data

We located 68% of the birth registration records and had matched pair data for 56% of records. This is a similar proportion to that identified by Goldberg and Morrison (55%), but lower than that in the study by Hare and colleagues (75%), both of which used similar methods.1,17 If the rate of mandatory birth registration of people from lower social groups were lower than that from higher social groups, the study would have systematic bias. We could find no evidence to support systematic bias, but an undetected bias would greatly affect our conclusions.

The strengths of this study lie in the relatively low geographical mobility of the population and the strict geographically defined catchment area for delivery of psychiatric services. Furthermore, the relative ethnic homogeneity of the study population avoided some potential cultural confounders. Not all patients were born in the catchment area, so we did not restrict the study to those whose parents were born and living within the index area. We identified the controls on a case by case basis from the district in which the patients' parents were living at the time of birth. We used temporally matched data because social class profiles have been shown to differ across decades and within countries.22

We used general population rather than psychiatric controls but we have no information on the outcome of the controls. Although it is possible that some developed schizophrenia, because the lifetime risk is 1% no more than four of the 352 controls are likely to have developed the disorder.

We had to rely on ICD-9 criteria for the diagnosis of schizophrenia rather than Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) criteria, which are more restrictive and would have given a lower rate of schizophrenia. A recent report suggests that rigid adherence to DSM-IV criteria may lead to underdiagnosis of schizophrenia.23 Furthermore, unless using the ICD criteria led to non-random misclassification, it will, at most, lessen the true strength of the association. Our data relate only to people who were admitted to hospital. However, during the study period over 98% of patients diagnosed with schizophrenia were admitted as inpatients.

Comparison with other studies

Our data contrast with the findings of Croudace and colleagues, who reported an increased risk of schizophrenia among people from lower social classes.7 A study of a northern Finland 1966 birth cohort, in which 11 017 people alive at the age of 16 years were followed up, found similar results to ours.24 The two studies are not directly comparable, however, because our case-control study is sensitive to systematic bias and their cohort study did not follow up participants beyond age 27 years.

The cumulative incidence of early onset schizophrenia was higher among young people in the highest social class (according to father's occupation) than among young people in lower social classes. Therefore, the consistently observed excess of patients in the lowest social class is probably accounted for by people with schizophrenia moving into lower social classes or not moving out of lower social classes.25

Effect of age on presentation

We found more than an eight year difference in age at first presentation to psychiatric services between the lowest and highest social classes. Although it is possible that schizophrenia has a different age at onset across social classes, this is unlikely. Similarly, it is unlikely that family doctors are selectively not referring patients from lower social classes. Use of health services differs between the social classes. For example, people in the lowest social class are least likely to seek early antenatal care; middle class parents are more likely to seek medical attention for their children; and working class adults are likely to be more ill than are middle class adults before seeking help.20 Schizophrenia is probably subject to the same social class effects.

One explanation for the effect of social class is that people from lower social classes find it more difficult to access services.22 However, Cooper found no support for this hypothesis,26 and in our region the community service is more focused on people from lower socioeconomic groups. Furthermore, once patients present to the psychiatric service there is little if any social class gradient in time to first diagnosis. Alternatively, people from the higher social classes may be better informed about schizophrenia or find it easier to identify deviations from the expected social and academic or occupational functioning.27,28 Other studies suggest that the beliefs and values of people in lower socioeconomic groups, such as their tolerance and acceptance of the behavioural and social aspects of the disorder18 and their sense of low control or powerlessness,29 may help to explain the observed socioeconomic inequalities.

A “contact” study found that low social class at birth increased the duration of untreated psychosis.30 If, as we suspect, people with schizophrenia from lower social classes are not presenting to family doctors, those with the least financial resources and opportunities for employment may be the most exposed to the adverse effects of untreated psychosis. This delay may, at least partially, explain why people from lower social class have a less favourable outcome.13 Efforts to reduce the duration of untreated psychosis through earlier detection should be particularly focused on people in lower social classes. In addition, researchers should be careful to adjust for social class at birth if using age at first presentation as a measure of age at onset of schizophrenia.

Acknowledgments

We thank the St John of God Order and the Stanley Foundation for their support and staff of the General Register Office, Dublin, for help in locating birth registration records.

Footnotes

Funding: Stanley Foundation and Hospitaller Order of St John of God.

Competing interests: None declared.

References

1. Goldberg EM, Morrison SL. Schizophrenia and social class. Br J Psychiatry. 1963;109:785–802. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Wiersma D, Giel R, De Jong A, Slooff CJ. Social class and schizophrenia in a Dutch cohort. Psychol Med. 1983;13:141–150. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Argyle M. The psychology of social class. London: Routledge; 1994. [Google Scholar]

4. Jones PB, Bebbington P, Foerster A, Lewis SW, Murray RM, Russell A, et al. Premorbid social underachievement in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. 1993;162:65–71. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Aro S, Aro H, Keskimäki I. Socioeconomic mobility among patients with schizophrenia or major affective disorder. A 17-year retrospective follow-up. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;166:759–767. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Warner R, de Girolamo G. Schizophrenia. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1995. [Google Scholar]

7. Croudace TJ, Kayne R, Jones PB, Harrison GL. Non-linear relationship between an index of social deprivation, psychiatric admission prevalence and the incidence of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2000;30:177–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Geddes JR, Lawrie SM. Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 1995;167:786–793. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

10. Hultman CM, Sparén P, Takei N, Murray RM, Cnattingius S, Geddes J. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for schizophrenia, affective psychosis, and reactive psychosis of early onset: case-control study. BMJ. 1999;318:421–426. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. Sham PC, O'Callaghan E, Takei N. Schizophrenia following pre-natal exposure to influenza epidemics between 1939 and 1960. Br J Psychiatry. 1992;160:461–466. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

12. Susser ES, Lin SP. Schizophrenia after prenatal exposure to the Dutch hunger winter of 1944-1945. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49:983–988. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

13. Saugstad LF. Social class, marriage and fertility in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1989;15:9–44. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Wynn SW, Wynn AH, Doyle W. The association of maternal social class with maternal diet and the dimensions of babies in a population of London women. Nutr Health. 1994;9:303–315. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Saxena S, Majeed A, Jones M. Socioeconomic differences in childhood consultation rates in general practice in England and Wales: prospective cohort study. BMJ. 1999;318:642–646. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Turner RJ, Wagenfeld MO. Occupational mobility and schizophrenia: an assessment of social causation and social selection hypothesis. Am Sociol Rev. 1967;32:104–113. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Hare EH, Price JS, Slater E. Parental social class in psychiatric patients. Br J Psychiatry. 1972;121:515–524. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Loebel AD, Lieberman JA, Alvir JMJ, Mayerhoff DI, Geisler SH, Szymanski SR. Duration of psychosis and outcome in first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149:1183–1188. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

19. Carbone S, Harrigan S, McGorry PD. Duration of untreated psychosis and 12 month outcome in first episode psychosis: the impact of treatment approach. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;100:96–104. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Department of Health and Social Security. Inequalities in health: report of a research working group. London: DHSS; 1980. [Google Scholar]

21. Central Statistics Office. Census of Population 1986 Classification of Occupations. Dublin: Stationery Office; 1986. [Google Scholar]

22. Birtchnell J. Social class, parental social class and social mobility in psychiatric patients and general population controls. Psychol Med. 1971;1:209–221. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Schwartz JE, Fennig S, Tanenberg-Karant M, Carlson G, Craig T, Galambos N, et al. Congruence of diagnoses 2 years after a first-admission diagnosis of psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:593–600. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Mäkikyrö T, Isohanni M, Moring J. Is a child's risk of early onset schizophrenia increased in the highest social class? Schizophr Res. 1997;23:245–252. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Dohrenwend BP, Levav I, Shrout PE, Schwartz S, Naveh G, Link BG, et al. Socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders: the causation-selection issue. Science. 1992;255:946–952. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Roberts K. School, parents and social class. In: Craft M, Raynor J, Cohen L, editors. Linking home and school. A new review. 3rd ed. London: Harper and Row; 1980. [Google Scholar]

28. Horwitz AV. Help-seeking processes and mental health services. New Dir Ment Health Serv. 1987;36:33–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Bosma H, Schrijvers C, Mackenbach JP. Socioeconomic inequalities in mortality and importance of perceived control: cohort study. BMJ. 1999;319:1469–1470. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Clarke M, Brown S, McTigue O, Gervin M, Murphy P, Waddington JL, et al. Duration of untreated psychosis in first episode schizophrenia and its relationship to premorbid functioning. Schizophr Res. 1999;36:38–39. [Google Scholar]


Articles from The BMJ are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group


Which marketing philosophy emphasizes interacting with customers on a regular basis and giving them reasons to maintain a bond with the company over time?

Chapter 1 Consumer Behavior 12E.

Which of the following is the study of the processes involved when individuals or groups select?

Consumer behavior is the study of the processes involved when individuals or groups select, purchase, use, or dispose of products, services, ideas, or experiences to satisfy needs and desires (Solomon, Russell-Bennett and Previte, 2013).

Is a person who identifies a need or desire makes a purchase and then disposes of a product?

A consumer is a person who identifies a need or desire, makes a purchase and then disposes of the product in the consumption process.

When consumers see only what they want to see don't see what they don't want to see is called ________?

Perceptual defense. When consumers see only what they want to see-don't see what they don't want too see is called. Interception.