Which of the following elements of temperament are linked with more optimal outcomes in adulthood?

Introduction

Observers since antiquity have suggested that children are born with a natural disposition or style of how they react behaviorally and emotionally to diverse physiological, psychosocial, and energetic stimuli1,2,3. This innate biological disposition was called a person’s temperament and originally referred to a person’s animal-like nature as manifest in habitual patterns of automatic activity and emotional reactivity (temper)1,2,3,4,5,6. When measured in this traditional way, temperament is moderately stable on average throughout a person’s life span, but can be modified by behavioral conditioning5,6,7,8. Despite moderate stability, there is also substantial complexity in the development of temperament, including multi-finality (i.e., a particular profile of traits in early childhood may have different outcomes later) and equi-finality (i.e., different profiles of traits in early childhood may have the same outcome later)9,10,11,12.

In contrast, the other aspects of personality that were presumed since antiquity to distinguish humans from ancestral animals were collectively called a person’s character. Kant defined character as what people make of themselves intentionally3. Put another way, character is the self-regulatory aspect of personality—that is, the way a person shapes and adapts responses to ever-changing external and internal conditions6. These self-regulatory processes include the executive, legislative, and judicial functions necessary for mental self-government and self-actualization of identity13. When measured in this way, the self-regulatory aspects of personality develop in incremental steps across the life span as people learn episodically from their personal, social, and cultural experiences what goals and activities interest them and why some goals may be more valuable and fulfilling than others13,14,15.

Many modern scholars and researchers have suggested a variety of empirical ways to distinguish temperament from other aspects of personality7,16,17,18,19,20,21,22. Others prefer to lump all aspects of personality together in profiles or sets of linear factors, suggesting that adult personality is essentially a culturally conditioned expression of childhood temperament23,24 despite their modest and complex patterns of empirical association10,11,12,13,14,25,26,27,28,29. Nevertheless, temperament involves emotional drives that are irrational and vary quantitatively in strength, whereas the self-regulatory components of personality have several properties that qualitatively distinguish them from temperament, as summarized in Table 1. Temperament has traditionally been distinguished from other aspects of personality by observations about its neurobiology, appearance in infancy, distinctive styles of automatic behavioral and emotional reactions, absence of intentional self-control or self-awareness, stability across the life span, heritability, and/or the evolutionary conservation of underlying molecular processes1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40.

Table 1 Features traditionally used to distinguish temperament from character (i.e., other aspects of personality)

Full size table

Unfortunately, the various criteria suggested to define temperament do not overlap fully and can even contradict one another at times. For example, high heritability or developmental stability has each been used as a criterion for temperament, which leads to disagreements about how to define temperament because they do not identify the same individuals7,8,41. Development in infancy is another criterion used to identify components of temperament, but not all cognitive-behavioral features that develop in infancy involve patterns of automatic reactivity that are highly conserved in the evolution of all animals: in particular, some aspects of executive attention and effortful self-control emerged only late in evolution among great apes42,43 but begin to develop in early childhood and then mature in steps across the life span17,19. Nevertheless, some recent temperament theorists have included such self-regulatory functions as temperament on the basis of their being heritable and beginning to emerge in early childhood17, even though they do not satisfy the other traditional distinguishing features of temperament.

Such definitional inconsistencies have arisen in part for the convenience of investigators with expertise in working with particular methods and samples. For example, some temperament investigators focus on cross-sectional assessments of young children and focus on whatever is present in early childhood. Others do longitudinal studies and focus on developmental stability, while others study inheritance in family and twin studies.

In addition, temperament investigators have sometimes relied too heavily on simplistic dichotomies like nature versus nurture, biology versus learning, and genes versus environment. Such dichotomies are totally inadequate to describe the complex phenotypic, genotypic, and environmental architecture of human personality25,26,27. For example, human beings have three distinct systems of learning and memory that are strongly associated with different components of personality: associative conditioning (i.e., how we learn to react automatically, including classical and operant conditioning), intentionality (i.e., what we learn as goals to purposefully seek, including self-direction and social cooperation for personal or mutual benefit), and self-awareness (i.e., when, where, and why we learn, including autobiographical memory with imaginative shifts in perspective taking underlying science, art, and spirituality)27,44,45,46. Each of these systems of learning is dissociable from the others, and each is moderately heritable, related to distinct brain circuitry, and their integration across the life span involves strong gene-environmental interactions in adapting to a wide variety of physiological, psychosocial, and energetic stimuli27.

We hypothesized that the distinction between temperament and character was more likely to be specified by identifying which system of learning and memory underlies temperament, not whether temperament is due to nature (genes and biology) rather than nurture (environment and learning)6,25,26. Consequently, the effective translation of knowledge about temperament requires attention to the complex architecture of personality along with knowledge of its evolution and complex patterns of development in individuals8,14,42,47,48,49.

Fortunately, we have recently used data-driven methods to identify single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) that map to 972 genes that explained nearly all the variability in temperament and character expected from twin studies in three independent samples of Finns, Germans, and Koreans25,26,27. These 972 genes include 245 associated with temperament only, 236 with character only, and 491 with both temperament and character. As shown in Fig. 1, the genes associated with temperament were more often protein-coding DNA genes than those associated with character25,26,27, which were more often long non-coding RNA genes or pseudogenes that influence the regulation of expression of protein-coding genes, coordination of the co-expression of sets of genes, and chromatin remodeling50,51,52. Most of the 736 genes associated with temperament are protein-coding genes involved in cellular processes of synaptic plasticity, associative conditioning, and related processes of stress reactivity and neurotransmission. The genes associated with personality were nearly always expressed in the brain (Supplementary Fig. 1). However, their brain functions frequently depended on interactions with genes for general housekeeping functions, such as the regulation of energy metabolism, cellular repair, and circadian rhythms, which occur in most or all cell-types and are associated with both temperament and character (Supplementary Fig. 1)53,54. These findings confirmed our hypothesis that the highly conserved molecular processes that regulate associative conditioning in experimental animals account for the heritability of human temperament. Our findings were confirmed in blindly independent replications by GWAS25,26 and by independent studies of gene expression during habit learning in experimental animals25,26,27.

Fig. 1

Which of the following elements of temperament are linked with more optimal outcomes in adulthood?

Distribution of biotypes of 972 genes associated with temperament and/or character, including long non-coding RNA (lncRNA), other non-coding RNA (ncRNA), protein-coding genes, pseudogenes, and others. Genes associated with temperament are more often protein-coding than those associated with character, which are more often genes with regulatory functions (lncRNAs and pseudogenes). Figure is reproduced from Fig. 4c of Zwir et al.27, Three Genetic-Environmental Networks for Human Personality)

Full size image

However, all our studies were conducted using the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) in which heritable dimensions of temperament are assessed by scales that measure individual differences in disposition to associative conditioning in response to signals of punishment (i.e., Harm Avoidance: fearful, shy), novelty (i.e., Novelty Seeking: exploratory, impulsive-aggressive), signals of reward (i.e., Reward Dependence: attached, approval-seeking), and intermittent reinforcement (i.e., Persistence: determined, ambitious)26. Therefore, here we will review the relations of our temperament measures with alternative modern measures of temperament. We will also review the habitual patterns of behavioral activity and emotional reactivity to various physiological, psychosocial, and energetic stimuli expected from traditional concepts of temperament with those observed for the molecular pathways we uncovered for temperament (i.e., Ras-MEK-ERK and PI3K-AKT-mTOR pathways). Finally, we will discuss the research and clinical implications of our findings about the complex genetics and biology of temperament for translational psychiatry.

Our new molecular finding and this review of several complementary lines of temperament research provide an excellent opportunity to build consensus within the diverse field of temperament research and practice, which has been lacking17,22. We hope to clarify controversies among temperament researchers who use different assessment methods and study different groups of subjects without losing the complementary insights that may be derived from the different strategies that have been employed in studying temperament. Establishing a consensus in which complementary lines of research may help us all to translate the extensive work that has been and is being done into a more comprehensive model of human development could facilitate a more realistic understanding of many complex aspects of temperament and personality that are important for understanding and promoting healthy development.

Partial overlap of concepts of temperament

Early descriptions of temperament focused on formal features of patterns of habitual behavioral activity and emotional reactivity that could be directly observed to response to environmental perturbations. In particular, seasonal variation in temperature (cold/hot) and rainfall (wet/dry) appeared to elicit individual differences in behavior and emotional reactions, and such observations gave rise to the ancient model of four temperament types2,3,55. These ancient temperament types were also distinguishable in terms of emotional style, valence of mood, intensity of arousal, and responses to rewards, novelty, and punishment, as summarized in Supplementary Table 1.

There has been no consensus about the optimal way to measure temperament based on these general distinguishing features, so a variety of measurement approaches have been used7,12,17. In order to relate the new molecular findings about the TCI to other models, we will first describe how the TCI measures temperament and then compare it to several alternative measures in order to assess the extent of their concordance conceptually and empirically.

TCI measures of temperament

The Temperament and Character Inventory was developed as a neurobiologically-based model of the evolution of learning by extending the research of Jeffrey Gray on the relationship of associative conditioning in experimental animals to adult human personality39,56. Put another way, the TCI measures individual differences in behavioral and emotional style, which Thomas and Chess5 described as how a person acts automatically from disposition and habit, rather than the intentional and self-regulatory aspects of personality that specify what, when, where, or why they act as they do.

Specifically, the TCI measures four temperament dimensions that have been empirically confirmed to quantify individual differences in associative conditioning and related human brain circuitry: Harm Avoidance (i.e., fearful, pessimistic vs. risk-taking, optimistic)57,58,59, Novelty Seeking (i.e., impulsive, excitable vs. deliberate, reserved)60,61, Reward Dependence (i.e., sociable, sentimental vs. detached, objective)58,61, and Persistence (i.e., determined, ambitious vs. easily discouraged, underachieving)62,63. High and low scorers on all the subscales of TCI temperament and character are given in Supplementary Table 2 to help relate TCI variables to the terminology of other measures. Harm Avoidance is an indicator of negative valence that measures passive-avoidance learning and increased sensitivity to behavioral inhibition in response to fearful stimuli, which is mediated by activation of the amygdala, subgenual cingulate cortex, and the insular salience network59,64,65. Novelty Seeking is an indicator of positive valence that measures behavioral activity to approach and explore novel stimuli66,67, even if they do not predict rewards61. In contrast, Reward Dependence is characterized by social attachment and approach to rewards based on a different pattern of activation of dopaminergic neurons in the nucleus accumbens and substantia nigra from that seen in association with Novelty Seeking61 and on oxytocinergic neurons in the hypothalamus68. Persistence measures individual differences in rates of extinction of intermittently rewarded behaviors in response to frustrative non-reward, which is mediated by activating a brain circuit connecting the nucleus accumbens, anterior cingulate, and ventrolateral frontal cortex62,63. Furthermore, these brain circuits for behavioral conditioning are modulated by regulation of the co-expression of sets of genes in the two major molecular pathways for response to extracellular stimuli: the Ras-MEK-ERK pathway and the PI3K-AKT-mTOR pathways25,26,27, as will be described after reviewing the relations of the TCI to other ways of measuring temperament in children and adults.

Strelau temperament inventory

Jan Strelau has produced a well-validated measure of temperament that is perhaps closest to the classical description of temperament by Kant3,4. Rather than speculating about temperaments being mixtures of various bodily fluids, Kant introduced the notion that temperaments could be recognized by observation of the formal characteristics of their behavior, which involve their energetic and temporal style rather than the content, situation, or goals of the behavior. Likewise Strelau observed that the most frequent and consistent indicators of temperament were its biological basis, presence since early childhood, appearance in both man and animals, and the formal characteristics of behavior as described by Kant4,69. Strelau found that the two formal characteristics of behavior emphasized by Kant (i.e., activity and emotional reactivity) had strong effects on the regulation of a person’s style and engagement in various behaviors and situations according to their stimulus value and psycho-physiological costs. Accordingly, his inventory, the Formal Characteristics of Behavior–Temperament Inventory (FCB-TI), measures self-reports of six formal characteristics of behavior in adults: Emotional reactivity (i.e., intense arousal), Briskness (i.e., quick tempo of response with mobility and flexibility), Sensory Sensitivity (i.e., low stimulus threshold), Activity (i.e., high energy level and social activity), Perseverance (i.e., persistence of action after cessation of reinforcing stimulation), and Endurance (i.e., tenacity despite long and intense stimulation). He has shown that his measures are moderately heritable and stable, and that they have strong correlations with other measures of temperament and personality, including the TCI, Pavlovian Temperament Survey (PTS), Buss and Plomin’s Emotionality–Activity–Sociability (EAS) inventory, the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire-Revised (EPQ-R)4,69, and the Revised Dimensions of Temperament Survey (DOTS-R) based on the features of behavioral style reported by Thomas and Chess to be moderately stable throughout childhood and adolescence70 (Table 2).

Table 2 Correlations (r × 100) of Strelau’s self-reports of basic energetic and temporal characteristics in adults with TCI (n = 28269 and other temperament surveys (n = 3924)

Full size table

TCI Harm Avoidance was the temperament most strongly correlated with the features of Strelau’s inventory, whereas none of the TCI character measures had strong correlations with the formal energetic and temporal characteristics of behavioral style. As shown in Table 2, Emotional Reactivity correlated strongly (r > 0.7) with TCI Harm Avoidance and EPQ Neuroticism, and moderately (0.7 > r > 0.35) with low scores on Pavlovian Mobility and Strength of Excitation and with high scores on negative emotions (distress, fear, anger) on the EAS. Activity is correlated strongly with EPQ Extraversion and moderately with TCI (low Harm Avoidance and high Novelty Seeking). Strelau’s formal characteristics were weakly correlated with DOTS-R measures of Thomas and Chess’s behavioral styles related to patterns of Adaptability (i.e., approach vs. withdrawal, flexibility vs. rigidity, mood quality) and Attentional Focus (i.e., low distractibility, persistence), but there were no significant correlations with Rhythmicity (i.e., regularity in sleep, eating, or other daily habits). Except for the weak correlations observed for Sensory Sensitivity, all of Strelau’s measures of the formal energetic and temporal characteristics of behavioral style have strong to moderate correlations with one or more TCI temperament traits and with factors in other tests, but not with TCI character traits. TCI temperament traits, but not character traits, also show rhythmicity, as discussed later.

The structural concordance of the four TCI temperaments with other measures of temperament has also been confirmed by their joint factor analysis with temperament as measured by the FCB-TI, EAS, and the DOTS-R in the Young Finns Study in 1997 when the 2106 participants were 20–35 years of age (Supplementary Table 3)71. Four factors corresponding to the four TCI temperaments were also identified by factor loadings over 0.5 of scales from the other tests: (1) Harm Avoidance along with FCB-TI Emotional Reactivity and EAS Negative Emotionality; (2) Reward Dependence along with EAS Sociability; (3) Novelty Seeking along with EAS and FCB-TI Activity; (4) Persistence along with DOTS-R Persistence. In addition, DOTS-R contained two factors with loadings over 0.5 that were not represented by the other tests: Rhythmicity (regularity in daily activities, sleep, and eating) and Flexibility, as was also observed by Strelau (Table 1).

New York and Colorado surveys of childhood temperament

The Colorado Childhood Temperament Inventory is a parental report inventory designed to assess the temperament of children from ages 1 to 6 years. It was derived by factor analysis from the features identified by Thomas and Chess in the New York Longitudinal Study and those identified by Buss and Plomin in their original Emotionality–Activity–Sociability–Impulsivity (EASI) inventory72. Recent work shows that such parent reports are only slightly biased by the personality and mood of the parents73. Using parent interviews and direct observations of children, Thomas and Chess measured temperament in terms of nine dimensions that appear early in childhood: activity, rhythmicity, approach vs. withdrawal, adaptability, intensity of reaction, threshold of responsiveness, quality or valence of mood, distractibility, and attention span/persistence5, which were later adapted by Windle in the DOTS-R for adolescents70.

In contrast, Buss and Plomin measured temperament in terms of four behavioral factors that they found to be heritable and developmentally stable7,18. The two systems overlapped extensively, especially in indicators of sociability, emotionality, and impulsivity. Six temperament factors appeared in the original merger of the two systems72. From the EASI, Emotionality (i.e., easily distressed or irritated), Activity (i.e., highly energetic), and Sociability (i.e., easily approached, warmly responsive, prefers presence of friends to being alone) were retained, but EASI impulsivity was divided into two components called Persistence (i.e., persevering, long attention span) and Soothability (i.e., easily calmed and distracted from distress) based primarily on items from the New York Longitudinal Study (NYLS). A sixth factor, Reaction to Food, was also originally contributed by items from the New York Longitudinal Study72, but later a factor for Shyness (i.e., inhibited and fearful around strangers) from the EAS was substituted as a more general indicator of fearful, inhibited behavior74. Impulsivity was dropped when the EASI survey was modified to form the EAS survey (see Table 2) because their measure of impulsivity was made up of heterogeneous features that were not all heritable or present in infancy18. Nevertheless, both Persistence and Soothability were retained in the modified CCTI48. The four factors of Emotionality, Activity, Sociability, and Persistence were only weakly correlated with one another, whereas Soothability (r = −0.42) and Reaction to Food (r = 0.25) were correlated with Emotionality (p < 0.001)72.

Thomas and Chess observed that “temperament individuality” was established by 2 or 3 months of age in the NYLS5. They identified three temperament subtypes with specific profiles (i.e., configurations of characteristics) that were relatively stable from 2 months to 10 years of age, which they called “easy”, “difficult”, and “slow to warm up” subtypes5,75,76,77 (Supplementary Table 4). Others have confirmed that the structure and levels of temperament scales and profiles are moderately stable from ages 1 to 5, and stronger thereafter, using a variety of instruments including the CCTI, EASI, EAS, DOTS-R, and preschool TCI or ratings of temperament profiles based on direct observations in early childhood28,48,78,79,80. However, as previously mentioned, there is also substantial complexity in the development of temperament scales and profiles, including multi-finality and equi-finality9,10,11,12,29. The original three prototypes described by Thomas and Chess accounted for only 65% of children, but more advanced prototype matching and clustering methods allow classification of nearly all subjects26,81. As a result of the early classification problem, many investigators preferred to emphasize continuous measures of temperament, usually three linear factors corresponding to negative affectivity/neuroticism, positive affectivity/extraversion, and effortful control/conscientiousness, as in Rothbart’s Child Behavior Questionnaire and Early Adolescent Temperament Questionnaire19. However, the frequent emphasis on dimensions rather than prototypes may be questioned because of the greater stability of profiles compared to their component scales in complex adaptive systems and developmental studies of temperament1,14, the greater value of a profile of the whole person for therapeutic interventions81, and now our finding that genotypic influences on temperament are acting on multidimensional profiles, not the individual traits commonly measured in inventories26. Fortunately, measurement of multiple temperament dimensions allows both quantification of individual traits and classification of multidimensional prototypes.

The six CCTI temperaments have been found to have strong correlations with the TCI scales in preschoolers, as shown in Table 348. The CCTI temperaments accounted for most of the variability in each of the four TCI temperaments (mR2 = 61–84%). However, CCTI scales did not significantly represent TCI Self-Transcendence (mR2 = 23%), which develops along with self-awareness later in childhood and adulthood. On the other hand, TCI scales did not significantly represent CCTI Activity (mR2 = 20%), which measures restless motor activity, even though they do for other scales of Activity that measure extraverted social activity or persistent and enduring activity (e.g., Table 1, Supplementary Table 3). The relationships between the TCI temperaments with parental reports of preschoolers were strong and similar to those observed with the formal characteristics of behaviors reported by adults in Strelau’s work: Harm Avoidance with CCTI Shyness (r = 0.82), Novelty Seeking with CCTI Emotional irritability (r = 0.64), Reward Dependence with CCTI Sociability (r = 0.74), and TCI Persistence with CCTI Persistence (r = 0.86). There were also weak to moderate correlations of TCI character traits with temperament as measured by TCI or CCTI, as expected due to the immature but developing functions of Self-directedness and Cooperativeness in self-regulation of Emotionality, Soothability, and Persistence.

Table 3 Correlations (r × 100) of preschool TCI with Colorado Childhood Temperament Inventory (CCTI) in parent reports on 64 children at age 30 months

Full size table

Adult temperament and personality inventories

The TCI scales also have a distinct and consistent pattern of relations with inventories designed to measure temperament or personality traits reported to be heritable and neurobiologically-based in adults (Table 4). Data are available about the Adult Temperament Questionnaire (ATQ) of Evans and Rothbart82, the Zuckerman–Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire (ZKPQ)83, the Emotionality–Activity–Sociability (EAS) Temperament Survey29, the revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire83, and the revised NEO Personality Inventory of Costa and McCrae84. The ZKPQ has measures of Emotionality (Neuroticism), Activity, Sociability, and Impulsive Sensation Seeking, so it is the alternative five-factor model for adults that is most similar in structure to the EAS and EASI temperament models for children and adults (see Table 4). These adult inventories also include self-regulatory components of personality with moderate to strong correlations with character traits of Self-directedness and Cooperativeness, such as ATQ effortful control, NEO agreeability/ATQ affiliativeness versus ZKPQ hostility, and NEO conscientiousness. Just as was observed with TCI temperaments in early childhood (Table 3), TCI temperaments in adulthood are closely related to other measures of adult personality traits, with correlations between 0.5 and 0.7: Harm Avoidance with measures of Neuroticism/Emotionality, Novelty Seeking with Impulsivity, Reward Dependence with Sociability/Affiliation/Extraversion, and Persistence with Activity/Conscientiousness. Although these relations are moderate to strong and indicate pervasive overlap in the overall content of different tests of temperament and personality, there are no simple one-to-one relations among the different tests, as can be seen in Tables 2–4 regardless of the number and content of factors or the age of subjects.

Table 4 Correlations (r × 100) in adults between TCI Scales and proposed measures of temperament or heritable personality dimensions derived by linear factor analysis

Full size table

In summary, TCI temperaments are differentially associated with other specific temperament scales using a variety of inventories based on a variety of characteristics, including the formal characteristics of behavior, onset in early childhood, developmental stability, and/or a heritable and neurobiological basis. However, these traditional criteria have not enabled investigators to specify the structure and content of temperament in a way that is discrete and distinct from other aspects of personality. The architecture of temperament and personality does not have the linear structure that is unrealistically assumed by linear factor analysis and classical psychometric test theory: there are significant relations among multiple components of one test with multiple components of other tests, rather than simple one-to-one relations between components of tests that focus on different characteristics (e.g., on formal energetic and temporal characteristics, on developmental stability, or heritability) regardless of age (Tables 2–4).

As a result of the complex internal structure of multi-scale temperament and personality inventories, most widely used inventories with documented evidence of criterion-related validity perform poorly when their structure is evaluated by confirmatory factor analysis85,86. Even in adulthood, development is complex and non-linear with substantial evidence of both multi-finality and equi-finality, as is expected for the behavior of non-linear dynamical systems involving learning to adapt to ever-changing conditions14,25,26,27,87.

Both automatic and self-regulatory aspects of personality are heritable41,88 and some self-regulatory aspects of personality begin to develop in early childhood19. Consequently more fundamental features of temperament than heritability and early appearance are needed to distinguish temperament from other aspects of personality. Therefore we will now discuss new molecular findings about the qualitative differences in systems of learning that have emerged at different stages in the evolutionary line of ancestors of modern human beings4,27,42,89 and that may distinguish temperament from other personality traits in a fundamental way that satisfies all the traditional concepts25,26,27,90.

Dispositions in habit learning as the fundamental basis of temperament

The temperament scales of the TCI were developed to measure specific constructs of associative conditioning based on data about the genetic structure of human personality in twins, phenotypic structure of habit learning by associative conditioning in humans and experimental animals, and the evolution of neurobiological mechanisms by which animals learn to adapt to changing conditions in their environment91,92,93,94. Initially the model was limited to temperament traits only, but has always included subscales to measure how broad dispositions are expressed in different situations92, as shown in Supplementary Table 2. Later observations revealed that people with any temperament profile could be healthy or unhealthy depending on character traits of Self-directedness, Cooperativeness, and Self-Transcendence, that were initially described on the basis of concepts from humanistic and transpersonal psychology6,13,56. We also found that temperament and character were equally heritable41, and hypothesized that they were distinguished by the distinct properties of brain networks that were equally heritable but involved in dissociable forms of learning and memory that had emerged at different times in the long evolutionary history of human beings: associative conditioning (i.e., classical and operant conditioning), Intentionality (i.e., self-directed and purposeful goal-seeking and cooperative behavior for mutual benefit), and Self-awareness (i.e., transpersonal or self-transcendent behaviors including creative imagination, mental time-travel, theoretical reasoning, and appraisal of values from a transpersonal perspective)25,26,27,42,95,96,97. Comparative analysis of neuroanatomy and emergent cognitive-behavioral functions in the ancestors of human beings suggested that temperament involved associative conditioning, which is highly conserved in all animals42. In contrast, brain functions for intentional self-regulation only emerged in higher primates, and self-awareness with creative capacities for art, science, and spirituality is present only in modern human beings42,45,89,95,98. These three brain networks normally interact in a coordinated manner99,100,101, but they are dissociable developmentally45,46,102 and functionally45,100,101,103,104,105,106.

A major limitation of earlier model-driven approaches to constructing temperament and personality inventories has been the tendency of people to fit their data to questionable assumptions of classical test theory using linear factor analysis. As we have just described in the prior section, the approach of fitting data to models has resulted in the failure to produce a consensus about how to measure temperament or how to distinguish it from other aspects of personality because people begin and end with different theories. Likewise, the tendency to fit genotypic data to models with the assumption that genes act independently of one another in the development of complex phenotypes like temperament and character is unrealistic because there is strong evidence of extensive gene–gene interaction for these traits107. In fact, prior estimates of gene–gene interaction in family studies of twins account for ~50% of the broad heritability of a variety of personality traits, with a range of 25–77%108,109,110,111,112, as summarized in Supplementary Table 5.

The model-driven approach to genome-wide association studies has failed to uncover the genotypic–phenotypic structure of complex traits and left most variability in complex traits unexplained by observed genotypes107. Instead of the estimates of 50% heritability of personality expected from twin studies, the heritability explained by SNPs has usually been ~10%, with a range of 0–21%113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120,121,122,123, as summarized in Supplementary Table 6. Therefore, we sought a data-driven method to test theories by fitting models to the data without restrictive and arbitrary theoretical assumptions.

We used a data-driven method called Phenotype–Genotype Many-to-many Relations Analysis (PGMRA) to identify SNPs that map to 972 genes that explained nearly all the variability in both temperament and character expected from twin studies in three independent samples of Finns, Germans, and Koreans25,26,27. Our machine learning approach124,125 uses the Non-Negative Matrix Factorization (NMF) method, which identifies multidimensional patterns within different types of data, such as quantitative or categorical phenotypes, genotypes, environmental variables, and/or voxels of neuroimages25,26,27,126,127,128. To uncover the natural genotypic–phenotypic architecture of a complex trait like temperament, PGMRA first dissects genome-wide data and uncovers a genotypic architecture composed of sets of SNPs shared by subsets of individuals (i.e., SNP sets), thereby allowing for complex genotypic information (such as gene–gene interaction and linkage disequilibrium) independent of any information about the phenotype. Next, phenotypic data are independently organized into natural sets of features, such as configurations of temperament traits shared by subsets of individuals (i.e., phenotypic sets); this allows for complex phenotypic interactions, such as heterogeneous temperament profiles, independent of any information about the genotype. Cross-matching of the two types of sets reveals multiple associations restricted to subgroups of individuals, thereby allowing for complex developmental phenomena, such as multi-finality and equi-finality. Other variable domains can also be integrated into the analysis, such as parental rearing, cultural influences, and other environmental exposures with or without measuring genotypes. The data-driven algorithm functions to extract and organize as much information as is available to increase the study power, so that moderately sized samples of people who are thoroughly assessed can be well powered107,128.

Our discovery sample was the Young Finns Study, an epidemiological study of 2149 healthy Finnish children followed regularly from 1980 (ages 3–18 years) to 2012 (ages 35–50)129. All subjects had thorough standardized genotypic, environmental, and phenotypic assessments, including administration of the Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) in 1997, 2001, 2007, and 20128,129. We replicated the results in two independent samples of 902 healthy adults from Germany130 and 1052 from Korea131,132 in which comparable genotypic and phenotypic features were available25,26. PGMRA was used to uncover the complex genotypic–phenotypic associations in the two replication samples (Germans and Koreans) independent of information about the discovery sample. The process used in the discovery sample was blindly and independently repeated in each replication sample without assuming homogeneity within or across samples107. We accounted for ethnicity in each sample by using the first three principal components for ancestral stratification of SNP genotypes25,26. Then matching of genotypic–phenotypic associations across samples was identified using parsimonious models that balance accuracy with model complexity, thereby avoiding over-fitting133. Models were learned independently in diverse samples to provide a stringent test of reproducibility despite complexity that might result from possible genetic, ethnic, cultural and environmental heterogeneity107.

We identified three clusters of people using the TCI temperament scales that measure individual differences in associative conditioning, behavioral activity, and emotional reactivity26. The three clusters corresponded closely to temperament clusters described by Thomas and Chess as “easy”, “difficult”, and “slow to warm-up”76,77. People in our “reliable” cluster resembled children with an “easy temperament” and adults who were conscientious extraverts because they were well-controlled in activity and were warm and calm emotionally. In other words, they were high in Reward Dependence (i.e., sentimental, friendly, approval-seeking), low in Novelty Seeking (i.e., deliberate, thrifty, orderly), low in Harm Avoidance (i.e., optimistic, confident, outgoing, and vigorous), and high in Persistence (i.e., determined). People in our “sensitive” temperament cluster resembled children with a “difficult temperament” and adults who are neurotic and unstable because they were under-controlled in activity and emotionally hypersensitive. Put another way, they were high in Harm Avoidance (i.e., pessimistic, fearful, shy, and fatigable), high in Novelty Seeking (i.e., impulsive, extravagant), and high in Reward Dependence (i.e., sentimental, friendly), so they frequently had approach-avoidance conflicts, rejection sensitivity, and disorganized attachments. People in our “antisocial” temperament cluster resembled children with a “slow to warm” temperament and adults who are socially detached, careless, and impulsive. That is, they were low in Reward Dependence (i.e., cold, detached, independent), low in Persistence (i.e., easily discouraged), and high in Novelty Seeking (i.e., extravagant, rule-breaking, but not inquisitive), which is frequently associated with maladaptive antisocial conduct.

We found 51 SNP sets that mapped to 736 gene loci and were significantly associated with one or more of the temperament sets. The neuronal functions and molecular processes associated with particular SNP sets and temperament profiles are shown in Table 5. Seventy-four percent of the identified genes were unique to a specific temperament profile, but 20 of the 51 SNP sets show substantial multi-finality (pleiotropy) in which at least 25% of carriers of the SNP set have different temperament profiles (Table 5). Such detailed data about the genotypic–phenotypic relations of temperament clusters are only available using the TCI. However, to facilitate consideration by readers familiar with other tests and to guide future investigation, the replicated TCI findings can be tentatively translated into profiles using scales measured by eight other major models of temperament and personality for which the relations with the TCI are known (see Supplementary Table S7). The genotypic sets distinguish people with distinct temperament profiles, so different descriptive models are hypothesized to capture the same clusters from various perspectives based on their correlations with the TCI: most traits in each system are at least moderately correlated with one or more TCI temperaments that differentiate the associated genotypic clusters.

Table 5 Neuronal functions and molecular processes associated with particular SNP sets and temperament profiles, including reliable (R), sensitive (S), and antisocial (A) profiles, and numbers of subjects and of genes mapped to each SNP set

Full size table

Put another way, the different models that have been developed for measuring temperament are like dialects of a common language with inconsistent accretions from their variable integration with character traits during development. The inconsistencies between these dialects are minimized when they are restricted to the multidimensional configurations that are associated with the common genotypic language of temperament. For example, each model of temperament identifies highly Harm Avoidant people as neurotic introverts (i.e., people who are high in Neuroticism, Negative Emotionality, or Emotional Reactivity) and low in Extraversion. However, Extraversion (i.e., positive emotionality) is a complex composite of low Harm Avoidance, high Novelty Seeking, high Reward Dependence, and some contributions from character traits (Tables 2 and 3). Consequently, Extraversion can indicate any of these TCI traits depending on its configurations with low Neuroticism (indicating low Harm Avoidance), high Impulsivity (indicating high Novelty Seeking), and/or high Sociability (indicating high Reward Dependence). Different inventories measure what they call high Activity in qualitatively different ways: it may involves extraverted social activity (i.e., high Reward Dependence, as in the Strelau FCB inventory), persistent and enduring activity (i.e., high Persistence, as in the ZKPQ or the Strelau FCB Inventory), or restless motor behavior, as in the EAS or CCTI, which is not consistently associated with TCI temperaments (see Tables 2 and 3). Like restless motor activity, TCI character traits are variably associated with TCI temperaments, as are ZKPQ Hostility and NEO Agreeability. Rothbart’s CBQ effortful control and NEO conscientiousness are composites of high TCI Persistence plus the self-regulatory character trait measured by high Self-directedness in the TCI. There is certainly loss of specificity for genotypic associations with such heterogeneous measures, but the multidimensional profiles should provide a useful tool for investigators without access to genotypic data about their own model, or for investigators to test the robustness of our genotypic findings with other models. We hope this information will also encourage more work with both individual dimensions and multidimensional profiles, which have complementary utility.

What then is the common genotypic language of temperament? Most of the identified genes were enriched in pathways activated by associative conditioning in animals, including the ERK, PI3K, and related protein kinase pathways, which are highly conserved in all animals (Fig. 2). When activated, the Ras-MEK-ERK cascade (also known as the mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) pathway) and the PI3K-AKT-mTOR cascade serve as the major cellular mechanisms for response to extracellular stimuli, including activation of processes that promote synaptic plasticity, associative conditioning, and long-term memory134,135,136,137,138. The cell-surface receptors for these pathways can be activated by a wide variety of physiological, psychosocial, and energetic stimuli that vary in positive and negative valence and in consequences for health and survival136,139,140. Changes in these pathways in response to associative conditioning occur in a coordinated manner with related processes including stress reactivity141, neuronal and glial growth142, and neurotransmission143. Both pathways converge on mTOR, which allows modulation of their joint action and circadian rhythmicity (Fig. 2)26,144,145.

Fig. 2

Which of the following elements of temperament are linked with more optimal outcomes in adulthood?

Cell displaying the molecular pathways containing genes associated with human temperament as measured by the Temperament and Character Inventory. The genes influence the Ras-MEK-ERK (MAPK), PI3K-AKT-mTOR, and Protein Kinase A, B, C pathways that regulate associative conditioning (reproduced from Fig. 2c of Zwir et al.25, Uncovering the Complex Genetics of Human Temperament)

Full size image

In summary, our findings suggest that individual differences in associative conditioning (habit learning, including classical and operant conditioning) may be the fundamental molecular mechanism for human temperament. Individual differences in associative conditioning provides a precise definition and causal mechanism that accounts for all the traditional concepts about temperament being distinguished from other aspects of personality by its formal behavioral style (how we learn) and emotional reactivity, which correspond to response patterns that are highly conserved in all animals, present in people from early childhood, and moderately stable across the life span.

However, these findings also open up many more questions for future study by investigators with diverse interests and skills. Temperament researchers have varied in whether they focused on individual traits or subtypes defined by profiles of multiple traits. We found that most of the genes for each associated with each temperament subtype were unique to that subtype, which suggests that the natural unit of measurement of temperament are profiles of multiple traits within an individual, not single traits that differ between individuals90. Nevertheless, activation of the genotypic sets leads to different behavioral responses in response to different environmental challenges, so this needs to be considered, as we begin to describe in the next section. The same person can carry multiple genotypic sets, so their individual traits may be a mixture of the effects of these multiple genotypic sets, as we have described elsewhere25,26 along with vignettes of the pure prototypes27. In addition, environmental influences during development can influence the development of temperament substantially by influences on the way the antecedents of temperament and character become integrated and self-actualized, as we have also begun to explore27. Identifying the fundamental molecular mechanisms underlying temperament is expected to help move its investigation forward in a more integrated way, and opens up many opportunities for translational research and practice. We will illustrate some basic questions that need more thorough study by available results regarding stimuli that allowed observers to recognize the distinguishing features of temperament in antiquity and that may still guide us in developing interventions to facilitate the healthy functioning of people by understanding their temperament.

Observations of temperament provide a direct window by which we can observe the powerful mechanisms that evolved in animals to allow rapid and effective adaptations to extracellular stimuli that are essential for the health and survival of all animals. Temperaments evolved in ways that help animals to adapt to naturally occurring variation in external and internal stimuli, which is essential for cellular proliferation and plasticity, resistance to degenerative processes (related to stress, injury, and aging), regulation of immune and inflammatory response, and maintenance of energy production, in addition to processes that mediate habit learning, emotional reactivity, cognitive flexibility, sensory sensitivity, and circadian rhythmicity, as shown for the functions of temperament-related SNP sets in Table 526,146,147,148. Consequently the molecular mechanisms underlying temperament may play important roles in susceptibility to the common diseases that burden modern society as a result of direct expression in particular organs and as a result of indirect influences mediated by lifestyle behaviors26,147,148,149,150,151,152,153.

The effects of key physiological and energetic extracellular stimuli on the Ras-MEK-ERK and PI3K-AKT-mTOR cascades related to temperament are summarized in Table 6. These stimuli are important regulators of adaptive responses to diurnal, seasonal, and climactic variation in conditions that require automatic adaptation in order to maintain cellular homeostasis, healthy functioning, and repair mechanisms. These stimuli correspond to changing diurnal and seasonal conditions to which animals must adapt for their health, reproduction, and survival despite changes between hot and cold temperatures154,155,156,157,158, light and dark luminosity145,159,160, and other conditions including exposure to electromagnetic fields161,162,163,164,165,166,167,168, variable supplies of water169,170,171,172 and nutrients172, and variable demands for physical activity173,174,175 and opportunities for sleep176,177,178. Under experimental or natural conditions, diurnal and seasonal rhythmicity in activity is associated with individual differences in TCI temperaments: people who are high in Novelty Seeking prefer to be more active late at night rather than in the morning179,180,181 and are more likely to have been born during the long photoperiod of summer than the short photoperiod of winter181,182. Furthermore, diurnal rhythms in activity are associated with seasonal rhythms in activity, emotionality, sociability, and body temperature180, much like the descriptions of distinguishing features of the classical temperament subtypes (Supplementary Table 1).

Table 6 Effects of physiological and energetic extracellular stimuli on temperament-related Ras-ERK (MAPK) and PI3K-AKT-mTOR pathways

Full size table

Translating the new genetics of temperament for research and practice

The first and major implication of the new genetic findings is a precise definition of temperament, which is really a fundamental need for good communication and incremental research progress within any scientific field. Based on the findings reviewed here, we propose the following definition: Temperament is the disposition of a person to learn how to behave, react emotionally, and form attachments automatically by associative conditioning (that is, rapidly and spontaneously, without conscious attention or reflection in response to changing internal and external conditions). Each part of the definition outside the explanation in parenthesis is essential: (1) temperament is the organization within an individual (i.e., a disposition, or set of distinguishing features) of how a person learns, not what, when, where, or why they learn; it involves the form and style of how a person learns; (2) the characteristic features involve what can be learned by associative conditioning, which include habitual patterns of behavior, emotional reactions, and attachments; (3) learning by associative conditioning in response to changing conditions is automatic and spontaneous (that is, without delay for conscious attention or reflection).

We propose that these criteria are necessary and sufficient to define temperament precisely. Our proposed definition is sufficient because it implies all the traditional criteria proposed for temperament, and it is necessary because the other criteria are non-specific when used individually or in combination. From this basic definition, it follows that the predisposition to temperament is innate and heritable, but its expression may change in response to associative conditioning, which can be modified by brain development or injury and by its integration with other systems of learning and memory related to other aspects of personality involving self-regulatory processes for intentional self-control and creative self-awareness. Associative conditioning is highly conserved in all animals, whereas intentional self-control emerged only in higher primates and self-awareness in human beings25,26,27. The integration of these systems is manifest in the complex and dynamic patterns of development that are observed for personality, language, art, and science across the life span of a person in response to changing conditions27.

We suggest that the proposed definition of temperament captures all the traditional concepts with specificity and precision, distinguishing it from other aspects of personality with which it becomes integrated during development. For example, a temperament can be unambiguously distinguished by heritable differences in behavioral conditioning; what is inherited as temperament is limited to the habit learning system, the component of procedural learning that is evolutionarily conserved in all animals. Cognitive systems for intentional self-control that emerged in higher primates may begin to interact with temperament from an early age19, but they involve fundamentally distinct molecular processes and brain structures than does temperament42,43. This definition yields the expected features of appearance in early childhood, prominence of basic emotions and automatic behaviors, and moderate stability over time, which also distinguish temperament from other aspects of personality, as summarized in Table 1.

An alternative definition is that “temperament refers to neurochemically based individual differences in the regulation of formal dynamical aspects of behavior22.” Reference to the formal dynamical aspects of behavior, as did Strelau (see Table 2), is useful to exclude character, but does not capture the rhythmicity and responsiveness to physiological stimuli (e.g., hot/cold, wet/dry, light/dark) that is prominent in classical concepts of temperament (Supplementary Table 1), the prominence of social attachments (sociable/aloof) (Tables 2–4), or in the molecular processes for regulation of diurnal and seasonal rhythms that we identified as fundamental features of the molecular pathways underlying temperament (Tables 2 and 6). We propose that only the form of learning (i.e., associative conditioning) and its evolutionary conservation are necessary and sufficient criteria for temperament because of the non-specificity of other criteria.

Several traditions that have approached temperament in different ways17 can now be recognized as converging and providing complementary information about how temperament and other aspects of personality develop across the life span. Defining temperament in terms of a specific and heritable form of learning makes it clear that distinctions between nature and nurture, biology and learning, genes and environment are inadequate. Temperament is the manifestation of a specific form of learning and memory, which is a non-linear dynamical process associated with complex patterns of inheritance and development. Individual differences in these adaptive processes are being investigated in terms of specific human brain functions using brain-imaging techniques96,97,126,183.

The temperament and character domains of personality do not function independently, so it is not surprising that investigators interested in temperament or personality often address similar questions. At times the overlap and interaction of temperament and character has led to confusion about what belongs to which domain because people function as whole organisms embedded in the world. We have identified the networks that integrate these domains and described their architecture, but there remains a need for further research to understand the integrative processes that bring the emotional reactivity of temperament together in balanced way with emotional regulation of character.

Personality research has closely aligned itself with temperament research by its emphasis on stability and use of similar methods based on assumptions of linear structure. However, it is crucial to recognize that personality has a complex biopsychosocial structure that is a product of interactions among multiple systems of learning memory that are dissociable functionally and developmentally.

Our findings about the complex genetics of temperament and character can best be understood from an evolutionary-developmental perspective. The evolutionary-developmental perspective helps to understand the adaptive functions of the molecular processes that distinguish temperament from other aspects of personality. The functions of the Ras-MEK-ERK and PI3K-AKT-mTOR pathways serve to maintain cellular homeostasis, healthy functioning, and repair of injury and degeneration despite diurnal, seasonal, and climactic changes in a person’s internal and external environment. Diverse stimuli can activate the molecular systems underlying temperament in coordinated ways that provide opportunities for effective interventions. However, there is great need for clinical trials to clarify how to use these natural stimuli effectively. As we begin to recognize that the psychobiological and genetic networks that regulate health and well-being correspond to systems of learning and memory, we have the opportunity and responsibility to develop and advocate an evidence-based approach to psychiatry that integrates knowledge about molecular, neurobiological, and psychosocial processes. The molecular aspects of psychiatry are only one level of organization that helps to open our eyes to the full multi-level organization of human functioning.

We have found that combining genotypic and phenotypic information does provide more information about health than does phenotypic information alone25,26. Consequently, genotypic panels for assessing the health propensities of people based on their personality are likely to be developed and offered commercially, as is being done for complex medical disorders. However, what has not been acknowledged by such commercial ventures is that the development of common disorders is highly complex and depends on the interaction of many sets of genotypic and environmental variables. Polygenic risk scores are not adequate for precise assessment of temperament because they rely on the average effects of genes acting independently, which can provide only weak and inconsistent information about personal health or risks of complex phenotypes in a specific individual (Supplementary Table 6)107. Even when complex phenomena (i.e., pleiotropy, epistasis, and gene-environment interaction) are taken into account, it turns out that the same genotypic profiles can be expressed in ways that are either healthy or unhealthy because of differences in the coherence of processes that regulate expression of genes and co-expression of sets of genes, often involving long non-coding RNA genes or a few “switch genes” that distinguish healthy and unhealthy character profiles25,27. For example, every possible TCI temperament profile can be either healthy or unhealthy, depending on a person’s character profile; there are average differences in risk between profiles, but nothing can be said about how healthy a particular individual is from their temperament alone14. Until we learn more about the processes that regulate the expression of protein-coding genes27, the additional costs and worries introduced by genetic testing of personality and/or common diseases may be unjustified when most information of practical value for personalized treatment planning is provided by improved phenotypic assessment at a lower cost. In addition, there are serious ethical issues concerning germline editing of the human genome to modify heritable human traits184. Our current reservations about the merits and dangers of introducing genotypic panels for enhanced personality assessment will need to be revisited once we gain more knowledge about the regulation of co-expression of sets of genes that lead to well-being and ill-being.

Psychopharmacology has already made substantial advances in developing treatments designed to target specific receptors, which can be an effective strategy when a small number of receptors cause a disorder consistently. However, when heterogeneous disorders depend on complex interactions among many genes and environmental variables, it is difficult or impossible to design interventions that are broadly effective and well tolerated.

Fortunately, we already know that the molecular mechanisms underlying temperaments evolved to help organisms adapt to naturally occurring physiological, psychosocial, and energetic stimuli, as was observed in antiquity. What is most important now is to consider how our molecular and clinical observations can be translated into useful interventions for disease reduction and health promotion. Use of cold (e.g., cryotherapy)185,186, heat (e.g., infrared light therapy)187, light exposure (e.g., bright light therapy)188,189, patterned EMF (e.g., transcranial magnetic stimulation)167, and lifestyle adjustments to optimize hydration, nutrition, exercise, and sleep190,191 have been widely advocated, but often produce weak and inconsistent results, particularly when there is inadequate motivation for change192 or limited understanding of the underlying mechanisms and the parameters critical for efficacy193,194.

Furthermore, there is extensive evidence that treatments of temperament are most effective when treatment addresses all three systems of learning and memory in a coordinated manner: behavioral conditioning, intentional self-control, and self-aware evaluation need to be integrated in order to be strongly and consistently effective in promoting health and well-being27,190,195,196,197,198. Put another way, relating a person’s current well-being to both their temperament and character provides powerful motivation for a person to change199. Fortunately, such thorough phenotypic assessments can also be expected to improve clinical trials by increasing study power in moderate-sized samples with stronger and more consistent results than have been obtained in poorly characterized and heterogeneous groups of subjects107.

References

  1. Kagan, J. Galen’s Prophecy: Temperament in Human Nature. (Basic Books, 1998).

  2. Avicenna. The Canon of Medicine. 1–710 (KAZI Publications, 1999).

  3. Kant, I. Anthropology from a Pragmatic Point of View. 1978 edn (Southern Illinois University Press, 1797).

  4. Strelau, J. The regulative theory of temperament: current status. Personal. Individ. Differences 20, 131–142 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Thomas, A. & Chess, S. Temperament and Development. (Brunner/Mazel, 1977).

  6. Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M. & Przybeck, T. R. A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 975–990 (1993).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Goldsmith, H. H. et al. Roundtable: what is temperament? Four approaches. Child Dev. 58, 505–529 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Josefsson, K. et al. Maturity and change in personality: developmental trends of temperament and character in adulthood. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 713–727 (2013).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Cicchetti, D. & Rogosch, F. A. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopatology. Dev. Psychopathol. 8, 597–600 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., Harrington, H. & Milne, B. J. Males on the life-course-persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways: follow-up at age 26 years. Dev. Psychopathol. 14, 179–207 (2002).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Rettew, D. C., Althoff, R. R., Dumenci, L., Ayer, L. & Hudziak, J. J. Latent profiles of temperament and their relations to psychopathology and wellness. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 47, 273–281 (2008).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Rettew, D. C. & McKee, L. Temperament and its role in developmental psychopathology. Harv. Rev. Psychiatry 13, 14–27 (2005).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Cloninger, C. R. Feeling Good: The Science of Well-being. (Oxford University Press, 2004).

  14. Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, N. M. & Svrakic, D. M. Role of personality self-organization in development of mental order and disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 9, 881–906 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Vaillant, G. E. & Vaillant, C. O. Natural history of male psychological health, XII: a 45-year study of predictors of successful aging at age 65. Am. J. Psychiatry 147, 31–37 (1990).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Dyson, M. W. et al. The structural and rank-order stability of temperament in young children based on a laboratory-observational measure. Psychol. Assess. 27, 1388–1401 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Shiner, R. L. et al. What is Temperament Now? Assessing progress in temperament research on the twenty-fifth anniversary of Goldsmith et al. (1987). Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 436–444 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  18. Buss, A. H. in Explorations in Temperament: International Perspectives on Theory and Assessment (eds Strelau, J. & Angleitner, A.) Ch. 3, 43–60 (Plenum Press, 1991).

  19. Rothbart, M. K. Temperament, development and personality. Curr. Directions Psychological Sci. 16, 207–212 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Trofimova, I. & Robbins, T. W. Temperament and arousal systems: a new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev. 64, 382–402 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Trofimova, I. An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality. Am. J. Psychol. 123, 467–480 (2010).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Trofimova, I., Robbins, T. W., Sulis, W. H. & Uher, J. Diverse perspectives on diversity: multi-disciplinary approaches to taxonomies of individual differences. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 373, https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0163 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. McCrae, R. R. et al. Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 78, 173–186 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kohnstamm, G. A., Halverson, C. F. J., Mervielde, I. & Havill, V. L. in Personality and Clinical Psychology Series (ed. Weiner, I. B.) 222 (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ, 1998).

  25. Zwir, I. et al. Uncovering the complex genetics of human character. Mol. Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0263-6 (2018).

  26. Zwir, I. et al. Uncovering the complex genetics of human temperament. Mol. Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0264-5 (2018).

  27. Zwir, I. et al. Three genetic-environmental networks for human personality. Mol. Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0579-x (2019).

  28. Caspi, A. et al. Children’s behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. J. Pers. 71, 495–513 (2003).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Pesonen, A. K., Raikkonen, K., Keskivaara, P. & Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L. Difficult temperament in childhood and adulthood: continuity from maternal perceptions to self-ratings over 17 years. Personal. Individ. Differences 34, 19–31 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Goldsmith, H. H. & Campos, J. J. in The Development of Attachment and Affiliative Systems (eds Emde, R. N. & Harmon, R. J.) 161–193 (Plenum, 1982).

  31. Ekman, P. Are there basic emotions? Psychol. Rev. 99, 550–553 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Sauter, D. A., Eisner, F., Ekman, P. & Scott, S. K. Cross-cultural recognition of basic emotions through nonverbal emotional vocalizations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 2408–2412 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Plutchik, R. The nature of emotions. Ameican Scientist 89, 344–350 (2001).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Izard, C. E. Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new paradigm. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2, 260–280 (2007).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Loehlin, J. C. Genes and Environment in Personality Development. (Sage Publications, 1992).

  36. Bouchard, T. J. Jr. & Loehlin, J. C. Genes, evolution, and personality. Behav. Genet 31, 243–273 (2001).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Loehlin, J. C. & Gough, H. G. Genetic and environmental variation on the California Psychological Inventory vector scales. J. Pers. Assess. 54, 463–468 (1990).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Darwin, C. The Expression of The Emotions in Man and Animals; with an introduction, afterword and commentaries by Paul Ekman; Essay on the history of the illustrations by Phillip Prodger. Ekman’s edition edn (HarperCollins Publishers, 1998).

  39. Cloninger, C. R. The genetic structure of personality and learning: a phylogenetic model. Clin. Genet 46, 124–137 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  40. MacLean, P. D. Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 42, 405–417 (1985).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Gillespie, N. A., Cloninger, C. R., Heath, A. C. & Martin, N. G. The genetic and environmental relationship between Cloninger’s dimensions of temperament and character. Personal. Individ. Differences 35, 1931–1946 (2003).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Cloninger, C. R. The evolution of human brain functions: the functional structure of human consciousness. Aust. N.Z. J. Psychiatry 43, 994–1006 (2009).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Allman, J. M. et al. The von Economo neurons in the frontoinsular and anterior cingulate cortex. Ann. N. Y Acad. Sci. 1225, 59–71 (2011).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Tulving, E. Episodic memory: from mind to brain. Annu Rev. Psychol. 53, 1–25 (2002).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Tulving, E. Multiple memory systems and consciousness. Hum. Neurobiol. 6, 67–80 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Levine, B. Autobiographical memory and the self in time: brain lesion effects, functional neuroanatomy, and lifespan development. Brain Cognition 55, 54–68 (2004).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Josefsson, K. et al. Parental care-giving and home environment predicting offspring temperament and character traits after 18 years. Psychiatry Res. 209, 643–651 (2013).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Constantino, J. N., Cloninger, C. R., Clarke, A. R., Hashemi, B. & Przybeck, T. Application of the seven-factor model of personality to early childhood. Psychiatry Res. 109, 229–243 (2002).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Zohar, A. H., Zwir, I., Wang, J., Cloninger, C. R. & Anokhin, A. P. The development of temperament and character during adolescence: the processes and phases of change. Dev. Psychopathol. 31, 601–617 (2019).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Fang, Y. & Fullwood, M. J. Roles, functions, and mechanisms of long non-coding RNAs in cancer. Genomics Proteom. Bioinforma. 14, 42–54, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gpb.2015.09.006 (2016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Quinn, J. J. & Chang, H. Y. Unique features of long non-coding RNA biogenesis and function. Nat. Rev. Genet 17, 47–62 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Barbash, S. et al. Neuronal-expressed microRNA-targeted pseudogenes compete with coding genes in the human brain. Transl. Psychiatry 7, e1199 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Faraone, S. V. The omnigenic model: implications for psychiatric genetics. J. Psychiatry Brain Sci. 2(S1), 1–2 (2017).

    Google Scholar 

  54. Hess, J. L., Akutagava-Martins, G. C., Patak, J. D., Glatt, S. J. & Faraone, S. V. Why is there selective subcortical vulnerability in ADHD? Clues from postmortem brain gene expression data. Mol. Psychiatry 23, 1787–1793 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Cloninger, C. R. & Svrakic, D. M. in Kaplan and Sadock’s Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry Vol. 1 (eds Sadock, B. J., Sadock, V. A. & Ruiz, P.) Ch. 26, 2126–2176 (Lippincott Williams & Wilkins, 2017).

  56. Cloninger, C. R. Temperament and personality. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 4, 266–273 (1994).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Corr, P. J., Kumari, V., Wilson, G. D., Checkley, S. & Gray, J. A. Harm Avoidance and affective modulation of the startle reflex: a replication. Personal. Individ. Differences 22, 591–593 (1997).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Corr, P. J., Pickering, A. D. & Gray, J. A. Personality and reinforcement in associative and instrumental learning. Personal. Individ. Differences 19, 47–71 (1995).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Pezawas, L. et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 828–834 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Houillon, A. et al. The effect of novelty on reinforcement learning. Prog. Brain Res. 202, 415–439 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Krebs, R. M., Schott, B. H. & Duzel, E. Personality traits are differentially associated with patterns of reward and novelty processing in the human substantia nigra/ventral tegmental area. Biol. Psychiatry 65, 103–110 (2009).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Gusnard, D. A. et al. Persistence and brain circuitry. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 3479–3484 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Cloninger, C. R., Zohar, A. H., Hirschmann, S. & Dahan, D. The psychological costs and benefits of being highly persistent: personality profiles distinguish mood disorders from anxiety disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 136, 758–766 (2012).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Yang, T. T. et al. Adolescent subgenual anterior cingulate activity is related to harm avoidance. Neuroreport 20, 19–23 (2009).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Paulus, M. P., Rogalsky, C., Simmons, A., Feinstein, J. S. & Stein, M. B. Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. Neuroimage 19, 1439–1448 (2003).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Naghavi, H. R., Lind, J., Nilsson, L. G., Adolfsson, R. & Nyberg, L. Personality traits predict response to novel and familiar stimuli in the hippocampal region. Psychiatry Res. 173, 94–99 (2009).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Lei, X. et al. Fiber connectivity between the striatum and cortical and subcortical regions is associated with temperaments in Chinese males. Neuroimage 89, 226–234 (2014).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Tost, H. et al. A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 13936–13941 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  69. Hornowska, E. Cloninger’s psychobiological model of personality and Strelau’s regulative theory of temperament–analysis of their associations in a Polish sample. Pol. Psychological Bull. 42, 71080 (2011).

    Google Scholar 

  70. Windle, M. Temperament and social support in adolescence: Interrelations with depressive symptoms and delinquent behaviors. J. Youth Adolesc. 21, 1–21, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01536980 (1992).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Puttonen, S. Common Elements of Five Temperament Models. MA thesis, University of Helsinki (2005).

  72. Rowe, D. C. & Plomin, R. Temperament in early childhood. J. Pers. Assess. 41, 150–156 (1977).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Kitamura, T. et al. Emotionality Activity Sociability and Impulsivity (EASI) Survey: psychometric properties and assessment of the Japanese version. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 3, 113–120 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Buss, A. H. & Plomin, R. Temperament: Early Developing Personality Traits. (Lawrence Erlbaum, 1984).

  75. Thomas, A., Chess, S. & Birch, H. G. The origin of personality. Sci. Am. 223, 102–109 (1970).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Thomas, A., Chess, S. & Birch, H. G. Temperament and Behavior Disorders in Children. (New York University Press, 1968).

  77. Carey, W. B. & McDevitt, S. C. Stability and change in individual temperament diagnoses from infancy to early childhood. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 17, 331–337 (1978).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Prior, M. Childhood temperament. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 33, 249–279 (1992).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Mathiesen, K. S. & Tambs, K. The EAS temperament questionnaire–factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 40, 431–439 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Rende, R. D. Longitudinal relations between temperament traits and behavioral syndromes in middle childhood. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 32, 287–290 (1993).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Westen, D. & Shedler, J. A prototype matching approach to diagnosing personality disorders: toward DSM-V. J. Pers. Disord. 14, 109–126 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Evans, D. E. & Rothbart, M. K. Developing a model for adult temperament. J. Res. Personal. 41, 868–888 (2007).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Zuckerman, M. & Cloninger, C. R. Relationship between Cloninger’s, Zuckerman’s, and Eysenck’s dimensions of personality. Personal. Individ. Differences 21, 283–285 (1996).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Cloninger, C. R. Personality and temperament: New and alternative perspectives. Focus (Am. Psychiatr. Publ.) 8, 161–163 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  85. Hopwood, C. J. & Donnellan, M. B. How should the internal structure of personality inventories be evaluated? Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 14, 332–346 (2010).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Ropovik, I. A cautionary note on testing latent variable models. Front Psychol. 6, 1715 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Hayes, A. M., Laurenceau, J. P., Feldman, G., Strauss, J. L. & Cardaciotto, L. Change is not always linear: the study of nonlinear and discontinuous patterns of change in psychotherapy. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 27, 715–723 (2007).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Lemery-Chalfant, K., Doelger, L. & Goldsmith, H. H. Genetic relations between effortful and attentional control and symptoms of psychopathology in middle childhood. Infant Child Dev. 17, 365–385 (2008).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Tulving, E. Episodic memory and common sense: how far apart? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond., B Biol. Sci. 356, 1505–1515 (2001).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Cloninger, C. R. & Zwir, I. What is the natural measurement unit of temperament: single traits or profiles? Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 373, https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0163 (2018).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Cloninger, C. R. A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatr. Dev. 4, 167–226 (1986).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Cloninger, C. R. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 44, 573–588 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Hansenne, M. & Ansseau, M. Catecholaminergic function and temperament in major depressive disorder: a negative report. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 477–483 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Cloninger, C. R. & Gilligan, S. B. Neurogenetic mechanisms of learning: a phylogenetic perspective. J. Psychiatr. Res. 21, 457–472 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Cloninger, C. R. & Kedia, S. in The Origins of Cooperation and Altruism Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects (eds Sussman, R. W. & Cloninger, C. R.) 63–110 (Springer, 2011).

  96. Van Schuerbeek, P., Baeken, C., De Raedt, R., De Mey, J. & Luypaert, R. Individual differences in local gray and white matter volumes reflect differences in temperament and character: a voxel-based morphometry study in healthy young females. Brain Res. 1371, 32–42 (2011).

    PubMed  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  97. Gardini, S., Cloninger, C. R. & Venneri, A. Individual differences in personality traits reflect structural variance in specific brain regions. Brain Res. Bull. 79, 265–270 (2009).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Povinelli, D. J. Folk Physics for Apes: the Chimpanzee’s Theory of How the World Works. (Oxford University Press, 2000).

  99. Ullman, M. T. A neurocognitive perspective on language: the declarative/procedural model. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 717–726 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Lee, A. C., Robbins, T. W., Graham, K. S. & Owen, A. M. “Pray or Prey?” dissociation of semantic memory retrieval from episodic memory processes using positron emission tomography and a novel homophone task. Neuroimage 16, 724–735 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Levine, B. et al. The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering: a prospective functional MRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 1633–1646 (2004).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Finn, A. S. et al. Developmental dissociation between the maturation of procedural memory and declarative memory. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 142, 212–220 (2016).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Nissen, M. J., Knopman, D. S. & Schacter, D. L. Neurochemical dissociation of memory systems. Neurology 37, 789–794 (1987).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Graham, K. S., Simons, J. S., Pratt, K. H., Patterson, K. & Hodges, J. R. Insights from semantic dementia on the relationship between episodic and semantic memory. Neuropsychologia 38, 313–324 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C. & Levine, B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2189–2208 (2006).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Fink, A. et al. Modulation of resting-state network connectivity by verbal divergent thinking training. Brain Cogn. 128, 1–6 (2018).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Zwir, I. et al. Uncovering the complex genetics of human personality: response from authors on the PGMRA Model. Mol. Psychiatry, https://doi.org/10.1038/s4138-019-0399-z (2019).

  108. Keller, M. C., Coventry, W. L., Heath, A. C. & Martin, N. G. Widespread evidence for non-additive genetic variation in Cloninger’s and Eysenck’s Personality Dimensions using a Twin Plus Sibling Design. Behav. Genet. 35, 707–721 (2005).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Eaves, L. J., Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., Hewitt, J. K. & Martin, N. G. Sex differences and non-additivity in the effects of genes on personality. Twin Res. 1, 131–137 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  110. Eaves, L. J. et al. Comparing the biological and cultural inheritance of personality and social attitudes in the Virginia 30,000 study of twins and their relatives. Twin Res. 2, 62–80 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Lake, R. I., Eaves, L. J., Maes, H. H., Heath, A. C. & Martin, N. G. Further evidence against the environmental transmission of individual differences in neuroticism from a collaborative study of 45,850 twins and relatives on two continents. Behav. Genet 30, 223–233 (2000).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  112. Finkel, D. & McGue, M. Sex differences and nonadditivity in heritability of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire Scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 72, 929–938 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Verweij, K. J. et al. Maintenance of genetic variation in human personality: testing evolutionary models by estimating heritability due to common causal variants and investigating the effect of distant inbreeding. Evolution 66, 3238–3251 (2012).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Vinkhuyzen, A. A. et al. Common SNPs explain some of the variation in the personality dimensions of neuroticism and extraversion. Transl. Psychiatry 2, e102 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  115. Power, R. A. & Pluess, M. Heritability estimates of the Big Five personality traits based on common genetic variants. Transl. Psychiatry 5, e604 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Genetics of Personality, C. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism, and the polygenic association with major depressive disorder. JAMA Psychiatry 72, 642–650 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Smith, D. J. et al. Genome-wide analysis of over 106000 individuals identifies 9 neuroticism-associated loci. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 1644 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  118. Docherty, A. R. et al. SNP-based heritability estimates of the personality dimensions and polygenic prediction of both neuroticism and major depression: findings from CONVERGE. Transl. Psychiatry 6, e926 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  119. Lo, M. T. et al. Genome-wide analyses for personality traits identify six genomic loci and show correlations with psychiatric disorders. Nat. Genet 49, 152–156 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  120. Okbay, A. et al. Genetic variants associated with subjective well-being, depressive symptoms, and neuroticism identified through genome-wide analyses. Nat. Genet 48, 624–633 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Luciano, M. et al. Association analysis in over 329,000 individuals identifies 116 independent variants influencing neuroticism. Nat. Genet 50, 6–11 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  122. Nagel, M. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for neuroticism in 449,484 individuals identifies novel genetic loci and pathways. Nat. Genet 50, 920–927 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  123. van den Berg, S. M. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies for extraversion: findings from the genetics of personality consortium. Behav. Genet. 46, 170–182 (2016).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  124. Zwir, I. et al. Dissecting the PhoP regulatory network of Escherichia coli and Salmonella enterica. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 2862–2867 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  125. Zwir, I., Huang, H. & Groisman, E. A. Analysis of differentially-regulated genes within a regulatory network by GPS genome navigation. Bioinformatics 21, 4073–4083 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Arnedo, J. et al. Decomposition of brain diffusion imaging data uncovers latent schizophrenias with distinct patterns of white matter anisotropy. Neuroimage 120, 43–54 (2015).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  127. Arnedo, J. et al. Uncovering the hidden risk architecture of the schizophrenias: confirmation in three independent genome-wide association studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 172, 139–153 (2015).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  128. Arnedo, J. et al. PGMRA: a web server for (phenotype x genotype) many-to-many relation analysis in GWAS. Nucleic Acids Res. 41, W142–149 (2013).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  129. Raitakari, O. T. et al. Cohort profile: the cardiovascular risk in Young Finns Study. Int J. Epidemiol. 37, 1220–1226 (2008).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Cook, T. B. et al. Latent infection with Toxoplasma gondii: association with trait aggression and impulsivity in healthy adults. J. Psychiatr. Res. 60, 87–94 (2015).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  131. Gombojav, B. et al. The Healthy Twin Study, Korea updates: resources for omics and genome epidemiology studies. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 16, 241–245 (2013).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  132. Sung, J. et al. Healthy Twin: a twin-family study of Korea—protocols and current status. Twin Res. Hum. Genet 9, 844–848 (2006).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  133. Deb, K. Multi-objective Optimization Using Evolutionary Algorithms. 1st edn, 497 (John Wiley & Sons, 2001).

  134. Luscher, C. & Malenka, R. C. NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation and long-term depression (LTP/LTD). Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Biol. 4, 1–16 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  135. Peng, S., Zhang, Y., Zhang, J., Wang, H. & Ren, B. ERK in learning and memory: a review of recent research. Int J. Mol. Sci. 11, 222–232 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  136. Tronson, N. C., Corcoran, K. A., Jovasevic, V. & Radulovic, J. Fear conditioning and extinction: emotional states encoded by distinct signaling pathways. Trends Neurosci. 35, 145–155 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  137. Brems, B. Spontaneous decisions and operant conditioning in fruit flies. Behav. Process. 87, 157–164 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  138. Rapanelli, M., Frick, L. R. & Zanutto, B. S. Differential gene expression in the rat hippocampus during learning of an operant conditioning task. Neuroscience 163, 1031–1038 (2009).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  139. Atkins, C. M., Selcher, J. C., Petraitis, J. J., Trzaskos, J. M. & Sweatt, J. D. The MAPK cascade is required for mammalian associative learning. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 602–609 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  140. Sakai, N., Ohno, H., Tomioka, M. & Iino, Y. The intestinal TORC2 signaling pathway contributes to associative learning in Caenorhabditis elegans. PLoS ONE 12, e0177900 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  141. Diamond, D. M., Park, C. R., Campbell, A. M. & Woodson, J. C. Competitive interactions between endogenous LTD and LTP in the hippocampus underlie the storage of emotional memories and stress-induced amnesia. Hippocampus 15, 1006–1025 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  142. Rapanelli, M., Frick, L. R. & Zanutto, B. S. Learning an operant conditioning task differentially induces gliogenesis in the medial prefrontal cortex and neurogenesis in the hippocampus. PLoS ONE 6, e14713 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Rapanelli, M., Frick, L. R., Bernardez-Vidal, M. & Zanutto, B. S. Different MK-801 administration schedules induce mild to severe learning impairments in an operant conditioning task: role of buspirone and risperidone in ameliorating these cognitive deficits. Behav. Brain Res. 257, 156–165 (2013).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  144. Swanson, J. M. et al. Etiologic subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: brain imaging, molecular genetic and environmental factors and the dopamine hypothesis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 17, 39–59 (2007).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  145. Ramanathan, C. et al. mTOR signaling regulates central and peripheral circadian clock function. PLoS Genet 14, e1007369 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  146. Mendoza, M. C., Er, E. E. & Blenis, J. The Ras-ERK and PI3K-mTOR pathways: cross-talk and compensation. Trends Biochem Sci. 36, 320–328 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  147. Fruman, D. A. et al. The PI3K pathway in human disease. Cell 170, 605–635 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  148. Steelman, L. S. et al. Roles of the Raf/MEK/ERK and PI3K/PTEN/Akt/mTOR pathways in controlling growth and sensitivity to therapy-implications for cancer and aging. Aging (Albany NY) 3, 192–222 (2011).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  149. Lee, M., Sorn, S. R., Lee, Y. & Kang, I. Salt induces adipogenesis/lipogenesis and inflammatory adipocytokines secretion in adipocytes. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 20, https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms20010160 (2019).

    PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  150. Liong, S., Barker, G. & Lappas, M. Placental ras regulates inflammation associated with maternal obesity. Mediators Inflamm. 2018, 3645386 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  151. Hintsanen, M. et al. Negative emotionality, activity, and sociability temperaments predicting long-term job strain and effort-reward imbalance: a 15-year prospective follow-up study. J. Psychosom. Res. 71, 90–96 (2011).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  152. Wessman, J. et al. Temperament clusters in a normal population: implications for health and disease. PLoS One 7, e33088 (2012).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  153. Hintsanen, M. et al. Cloninger’s temperament traits and preclinical atherosclerosis: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. J. Psychosom. Res. 67, 77–84 (2009).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  154. Chan, E. Y., Stang, S. L., Bottorff, D. A. & Stone, J. C. Hypothermic stress leads to activation of Ras-Erk signaling. J. Clin. Invest 103, 1337–1344 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Molina, J. R. & Adjei, A. A. The Ras/Raf/MAPK pathway. J. Thorac. Oncol. 1, 7–9 (2006).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  156. Yoshihara, T. et al. Heat stress activates the Akt/mTOR signalling pathway in rat skeletal muscle. Acta Physiol. (Oxf.) 207, 416–426 (2013).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  157. Kakigi, R. et al. Heat stress enhances mTOR signaling after resistance exercise in human skeletal muscle. J. Physiol. Sci. 61, 131–140 (2011).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  158. Gomes, A. P. & Adjei, A. A. A nexus for cellular homeostasis: the interplay between metabolic and signal transduction pathways. Curr. Opin. Biotechnol. 34, 110–117 (2015).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  159. Zhang, K. et al. Light-mediated kinetic control reveals the temporal effect of the Raf/MEK/ERK pathway in PC12 cell neurite outgrowth. PLoS ONE 9, e92917 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  160. Krishnamurthy, V. V. et al. Light-mediated reversible modulation of the mitogen-activated protein kinase pathway during cell differentiation and xenopus embryonic development. J. Vis. Exp. https://doi.org/10.3791/55823 (2017).

  161. Caraglia, M. et al. Electromagnetic fields at mobile phone frequency induce apoptosis and inactivation of the multi-chaperone complex in human epidermoid cancer cells. J. Cell Physiol. 204, 539–548 (2005).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  162. Pall, M. L. Microwave frequency electromagnetic fields (EMFs) produce widespread neuropsychiatric effects including depression. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 75, 43–51 (2016).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  163. Altun, G. et al. Protective effects of melatonin and omega-3 on the hippocampus and the cerebellum of adult Wistar albino rats exposed to electromagnetic fields. J. Microsc Ultrastruct. 5, 230–241 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  164. Buckner, C. A., Buckner, A. L., Koren, S. A., Persinger, M. A. & Lafrenie, R. M. Exposure to a specific time-varying electromagnetic field inhibits cell proliferation via cAMP and ERK signaling in cancer cells. Bioelectromagnetics 39, 217–230 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  165. Rouleau, N. & Dotta, B. T. Electromagnetic fields as structure-function zeitgebers in biological systems: environmental orchestrations of morphogenesis and consciousness. Front Integr. Neurosci. 8, 84 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  166. Singh, A. et al. Personalized repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation temporarily alters default mode network in healthy subjects. Sci. Rep. 9, 5631 (2019).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  167. Siddiqi, S. H., Chockalingam, R., Cloninger, C. R., Lenze, E. J. & Cristancho, P. Use of the temperament and character inventory to predict response to repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for major depression. J. Psychiatr. Pr. 22, 193–202 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  168. Wang, J. Q. & Mao, L. The ERK pathway: molecular mechanisms and treatment of depression. Mol. Neurobiol. (2019).

  169. Schliess, F., Richter, L., vom Dahl, S. & Haussinger, D. Cell hydration and mTOR-dependent signalling. Acta Physiol. 187, 223–229 (2006).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  170. Vitalini, M. W. et al. Circadian rhythmicity mediated by temporal regulation of the activity of p38 MAPK. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 18223–18228 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  171. Dubbelhuis, P. F. & Meijer, A. J. Hepatic amino acid-dependent signaling is under the control of AMP-dependent protein kinase. FEBS Lett. 521, 39–42 (2002).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  172. Howell, J. J. et al. Metformin inhibits hepatic mTORC1 signaling via dose-dependent mechanisms involving AMPK and the TSC complex. Cell Metab. 25, 463–471 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  173. Wright, D. C. et al. Exercise-induced mitochondrial biogenesis begins before the increase in muscle PGC-1alpha expression. J. Biol. Chem. 282, 194–199 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  174. Wright, D. C., Geiger, P. C., Han, D. H., Jones, T. E. & Holloszy, J. O. Calcium induces increases in peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor gamma coactivator-1alpha and mitochondrial biogenesis by a pathway leading to p38 mitogen-activated protein kinase activation. J. Biol. Chem. 282, 18793–18799 (2007).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  175. Perry, C. G. R. Mitochondrial adaptations to exercise in human skeletal muscle: a possible role for cristae density as a determinant of muscle fitness. J. Physiol. 595, 2773–2774 (2017).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  176. Miao, S., Liu, Y., Zhang, L., Shan, M. & Miao, Z. Effects of MAPK/ERK pathway on learning and memory in sleep deprivation rats. Int. J. Clin. Exp. Med. 11, 9160–9166 (2018).

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  177. Perogamvros, L. et al. Increased reward-related behaviors during sleep and wakefulness in sleepwalking and idiopathic nightmares. PLoS ONE 10, e0134504 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  178. Mikhail, C., Vaucher, A., Jimenez, S. & Tafti, M. ERK signaling pathway regulates sleep duration through activity-induced gene expression during wakefulness. Sci. Signal. 10, https://doi.org/10.1126/scisignal.aai9219 (2017).

    PubMed  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  179. Randler, C. & Saliger, L. Relationship between morningness-eveningness and temperament and character dimensions in adolescents. Personal. Individ. Differences 50, 148–152 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  180. Johansson, C. et al. Circadian clock-related polymorphisms in seasonal affective disorder and their relevance to diurnal preference. Neuropsychopharmacology 28, 734–739 (2003).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  181. Chotai, J. Novelty seekers and summer-borns are likely to be low in morningness. Eur. Psychiatry 20, 307 (2005).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  182. Chotai, J., Joukamaa, M., Taanila, A., Lichtermann, D. & Miettunen, J. Novelty seeking among adult women is lower for the winter borns compared to the summer borns: replication in a large Finnish birth cohort. Compr. Psychiatry 50, 562–566 (2009).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  183. Van Schuerbeek, P., Baeken, C., Luypaert, R., De Raedt, R. & De Mey, J. Does the amygdala response correlate with the personality trait ‘harm avoidance’ while evaluating emotional stimuli explicitly? Behav. Brain Funct. 10, 18 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  184. Lander, E. S. et al. Adopt a moratorium on heritable genome editing. Nature 567, 165–168 (2019).

    CAS  Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  185. Bleakley, C. M., Bieuzen, F., Davison, G. W. & Costello, J. T. Whole-body cryotherapy: empirical evidence and theoretical perspectives. Open Access J. Sports Med. 5, 25–36 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  186. Costello, J. T. et al. Whole-body cryotherapy (extreme cold air exposure) for preventing and treating muscle soreness after exercise in adults. Cochrane Database SystRev CD010789, https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD010789.pub2 (2015).

  187. Shui, S., Wang, X., Chiang, J. Y. & Zheng, L. Far-infrared therapy for cardiovascular, autoimmune, and other chronic health problems: a systematic review. Exp. Biol. Med. (Maywood) 240, 1257–1265 (2015).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  188. Oldham, M. A. & Ciraulo, D. A. Bright light therapy for depression: a review of its effects on chronobiology and the autonomic nervous system. Chronobiol. Int 31, 305–319 (2014).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  189. Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. & Lingjaerde, O. Response to light therapy in seasonal affective disorder: personality disorders and temperament as predictors of outcome. J. Affect Disord. 41, 101–110 (1996).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  190. Nigg, C. R. et al. Stages of change across ten health risk behaviors for older adults. Gerontologist 39, 473–482 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  191. Prochaska, J. O. et al. Multiple risk expert systems interventions: impact of simultaneous stage-matched expert system interventions for smoking, high-fat diet, and sun exposure in a population of parents. Health Psychol. 23, 503–516 (2004).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  192. Kobau, R., Sniezek, J., Zack, M. M., Lucas, R. E. & Adam, B. Well-being assessment: An evaluation of well-being scales for public health and population stimates of well-being among US adults. Appl. Psychol. 2, 272–297 (2010).

    Google Scholar 

  193. Krueger, M., Costello, J. T., Achtzehn, S., Dittmar, K. H. & Mester, J. Whole-body cryotherapy (−110 degrees C) following high-intensity intermittent exercise does not alter hormonal, inflammatory or muscle damage biomarkers in trained males. Cytokine 113, 277–284 (2019).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  194. Polidori, G. et al. Should whole body cryotherapy sessions be differentiated between women and men? A preliminary study on the role of the body thermal resistance. Med Hypotheses 120, 60–64 (2018).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  195. Cloninger, C. R. & Cloninger, K. M. People create health: Effective health promotion is a creative process. International. J. Pers.-centered Med. 3, 114–122 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  196. Mirowsky, J. & Ross, C. E. Education, personal control, lifestyle and health: a human capital hypothesis. Res. Aging 20, 415–449 (1998).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  197. Hayes, S. A., Orsillo, S. M. & Roemer, L. Changes in proposed mechanisms of action during an acceptance-based behavior therapy for generalized anxiety disorder. Behav. Res Ther. 48, 238–245 (2010).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  198. Cloninger, C. R. Person-centered health promotion in chronic disease. Int. J. Pers.-centered Med. 3, 5–12 (2013).

    Google Scholar 

  199. Cloninger, C. R. & Cloninger, K. M. Person-centered therapeutics. Int. J. Pers.-centered Med. 1, 43–52 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Which of the following is true about the stability of attachment styles in adulthood?

Which of the following is true about the stability of attachment styles in adulthood? Attachment styles are somewhat stable in adulthood, but adults can change their attachment thinking and behavior.

What percentage of adults describe themselves as securely attached?

Estimates vary, but research suggests that 50 to 60 percent of people have a secure attachment style, so there's a good chance of finding a romantic partner who can help you overcome your insecurities.

What is it called when our own attitudes and behavior feel supported because someone else's attitudes and behaviors are similar to our own quizlet?

Lower levels of depression, Lower body mass index, lower levels of biological risk factors. What is it called when our own attitudes and behavior are supported and validated because someone else's attitudes and behaviors are similar to our own? Consensual Validation.

Which parenting style tends to result in children who become unhappy and lack self control?

Permissive parenting often results in children who rank low in happiness and self-regulation. These children are more likely to experience problems with authority and tend to perform poorly in school. Uninvolved parenting styles rank lowest across all life domains.