Which of the following effect is most closely related to the cognitive hypothesis of reminiscence bump?

The peaks of life: The differential temporal locations of the reminiscence bump across disparate cueing methods

Author links open overlay panelJonathanKoppelPersonEnvelopeDortheBerntsenEnvelope

Show
Show moreNavigate Down

ShareShare

Cited ByCite

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.11.004Get rights and content

Abstract

The reminiscence bump has generally been assessed through either (1) the cue word method, or (2) several related methods which we refer to under the umbrella of the important memories method. Here we provide a review of the literature demonstrating that the temporal location of the bump varies systematically according to cueing method, with the mean range of the bump located from 8.7 to 22.5 years of age for word-cued memories, versus 15.1 to 27.9 for important memories. This finding has hitherto been under-acknowledged, as existing theoretical accounts of the bump generally hold its location to be stable across cueing methods. We therefore re-evaluate existing theoretical accounts of the bump in light of these varying locations, addressing each account's consistency with (1) the respective bumps found through each method taken individually, and (2) the sensitivity of the bump's location to cueing method.

Introduction

The reminiscence bump refers to the disproportionate number of autobiographical memories, in middle-aged and older adults, dating from adolescence and early adulthood (Rubin, Wetzler, & Nebes, 1986). Because this distribution breaks away from the standard forgetting function (e.g., Ebbinghaus, 1964, Rubin and Wenzel, 1996), it has been considered a distinctive feature of autobiographical memory and one of its defining characteristics. Indeed, the bump is mentioned in most, if not all, introductory textbooks covering the field (e.g., Eysenck and Keane, 2010, Goldstein, 2008, Rathbone et al., 2012).

The distribution of autobiographical memories across the lifespan has most often been assessed through one of two broad classes of cueing techniques. We will refer to the first technique, developed by Crovitz and Schiffman (1974) as a modification of a procedure used by Galton (1879), as the cue word method. Here, participants generate memories in association to cue words (for subsequent studies employing this technique, see, e.g., Janssen et al., 2011, Rubin and Schulkind, 1997a, Rubin and Schulkind, 1997b, Schuman and Corning, 2014). In the second technique, which we will refer to under the umbrella of the important memories method, participants are asked to report particularly notable memories. Examples of memory assessments focusing on important memories include, for instance, queries for important memories (or the most important memories) from participants’ lives (e.g., Cuervo-Lombard et al., 2007, Glück and Bluck, 2007, Rubin and Schulkind, 1997b) and queries for especially vivid memories (e.g., Benson et al., 1992, Fitzgerald, 1988, Robinson and Taylor, 1998). We also include in this category cases in which participants were asked to simply freely recall autobiographical memories, with no explicit instruction that these memories should be important (e.g., Conway and Holmes, 2004, Demiray et al., 2009, Rabbitt and Winthorpe, 1988), as we suspect that the search process triggered through such free-recall methods is far more similar to the search process triggered through the important memory method than the cue word method (see below).

The salient distinction between these two classes of cueing techniques concerns the retrieval strategies required by each. The cue word method is held to instigate an associative, bottom-up search process (Crovitz & Schiffman, 1974), while the important memories method involves a strategic, top-down search, structured around important memories in particular. This has implications for the nature of the autobiographical memories produced through each method, with the cue word method yielding a putatively unbiased sampling of autobiographical memories over the lifespan (Crovitz & Schiffman, 1974), while the important memories method yields a focus on the most significant memories of one's life. Memories elicited through the important memory method are, correspondingly, more closely related to meaning-making processes and personal identity (e.g., Glück & Bluck, 2007).

It is perhaps unsurprising, then, that neither the bump, nor the broader distribution of memories across the lifespan, is identical across these two cueing methods. First, the bump is larger in the important memories method. Second, word-cued memories, correspondingly, exhibit a sizable recency effect which is, at best, drastically attenuated in important memories (Fitzgerald, 1988, Fromholt et al., 2003, Rubin and Schulkind, 1997b). Here we draw attention to another difference between the bumps found through each method, one which has been little studied. This difference concerns the temporal location of the bump. As we will document in the current review, the location of the bump varies across the cue word and important memories methods. These disparate locations of the bump hold implications for theoretical accounts of the effect. Therefore, we will go on to re-evaluate existing accounts in light of this under-acknowledged sensitivity of the location of the bump to cueing method.

The disparate locations of the bump across cueing methods has generally gone unrecognized or unacknowledged in the literature, as authors of textbooks in cognitive psychology (e.g., Eysenck and Keane, 2010, Goldstein, 2008, Rathbone et al., 2012) and academic articles (e.g., Bohn and Berntsen, 2011, Dickson et al., 2011, Habermas, 2007, Koppel and Berntsen, 2014, Morrison and Conway, 2010, Schrauf and Hoffman, 2007, Shimizu et al., 2012, Thomsen et al., 2011, Webster and Gould, 2007) usually describe the bump as a unitary phenomenon, most often citing the ages of approximately 15–30 as representing the bump period.

To be sure, there has been some acknowledgement of the divergent locations of the bump. Most notably, Rubin and Schulkind (1997b) culled autobiographical memories through both cue words and by asking participants to report five of the most important events of their lives. They found that, while the bump for word-cued memories stretched from ages 10 to 29, the bump for most important memories was concentrated in the 20–29 range (for other references to this finding, see also Janssen et al., 2011, Janssen and Murre, 2008, Janssen et al., 2011, Kawasaki et al., 2011, Maki et al., 2013).

However, to this point, no systematic reviews have followed up on these isolated findings and observations. The lack of a systematic review illustrating the sensitivity of the bump's location to cueing method may be why most researchers fail to note this effect.

Additionally, prior researchers have not precisely isolated the age ranges over which the individual bumps in each method have been found across the literature, nor have they fully grappled with the theoretical implications of these divergent bumps. In light of these considerations, there is a need for: (1) A corrective to the widespread oversimplification of the bump's location as being unitary, including a systematic demonstration of the actual location of the bump as found through both the cue word and important memories methods, and (2) a thorough reckoning of the implications of the varying locations of the bump for existing theoretical accounts of the effect.

Section snippets

The temporal location of the reminiscence bump

As a means of identifying the temporal location of the reminiscence bump according to the two methods reviewed above, we have listed all the relevant papers which, to our knowledge, have probed for the bump thus far (Table 1, Table 2). Table 1 lists articles in which the cue word method was employed, and Table 2 lists articles employing the important memories method. Table 1 illustrates the type of cue word used in each study (with the most common being nouns; e.g., bar, factory, chair), the

Implications for theoretical accounts of the reminiscence bump

The identification of the differential temporal locations of the bump across the cue word and important memory methods calls for a reconsideration of existing accounts of the reminiscence bump phenomenon. It particular, it suggests that two specific criteria need to be applied in weighing each account of the bump. First, as Janssen and colleagues have noted (Janssen et al., 2011, Janssen et al., 2011, Kawasaki et al., 2011, Maki et al., 2013), not every account of the bump necessarily applies

Discussion

In this review, we have attempted to evaluate each of the existing accounts of the reminiscence bump in terms of (1) its consistency with the respective bumps in word-cued memories and important memories taken individually, in light of each bump's temporal location, and (2) its consistency with the disparate temporal locations of the bump across these two methods. The latter criterion effectively refers to the extent to which a given theory stresses processes at retrieval as underlying the

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Danish National Research Foundation, under Grant DNRF 93. We thank Sinué Salgado for his assistance with the figures, and Annette Bohn, Christina Lundsgaard Ottsen, Alejandra Zaragoza Scherman, and Müge Özbek for their comments on an earlier draft of this article.

References (109)

  • R.W. Schrauf et al.

    Bilingual autobiographical memory in older adult immigrants: A test of cognitive explanations of the reminiscence bump and the linguistic encoding of memories

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (1998)

  • S. Raffard et al.

    Narrative identity in schizophrenia

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2010)

  • C.M. Morrison et al.

    First words and first memories

    Cognition

    (2010)

  • S.M.J. Janssen et al.

    The relation between verbal and visuospatial memory and autobiographical memory

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2015)

  • A. Grysman et al.

    The time travelling self: Comparing self and other in narratives of past and future events

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2013)

  • I.M. Davison et al.

    Regret as autobiographical memory

    Cognitive Psychology

    (2008)

  • M.A. Conway

    Memory and the self

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2005)

  • S. Chu et al.

    Long live Proust: The odour-cued autobiographical memory bump

    Cognition

    (2000)

  • J. Cerella et al.

    The rise and fall in information-processing rates over the life span

    Acta Psychologica

    (1994)

  • W.F. Brewer et al.

    Role of schemata in memory for places

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1981)

  • D. Berntsen et al.

    Involuntary (spontaneous) mental time travel into the past and future

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2008)

  • R.C. Anderson et al.

    Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective

    Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior

    (1978)

  • N. Alea et al.

    The bumps in Trinidadian life: Reminiscence bumps for positive and negative life events

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2014)

  • F.C. Bartlett

    Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology

    (1932)

  • K.A. Benson et al.

    Socio-historical context and autobiographical memories: Variations in the reminiscence phenomenon

  • D. Berntsen et al.

    Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking

    Memory & Cognition

    (2010)

  • D. Berntsen et al.

    Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the lifespan: The retention of happy, sad, traumatic, and involuntary memories

    Psychology and Aging

    (2002)

  • D. Berntsen et al.

    Cultural life scripts structure recall from autobiographical memory

    Memory & Cognition

    (2004)

  • D. Berntsen et al.

    Two versions of life: Emotionally negative and positive life events have different roles in the organization of life story and identity

    Emotion

    (2011)

  • S. Bluck et al.

    The life story schema

    Motivation and Emotion

    (2000)

  • A. Bohn

    Generational differences in cultural life scripts and life story memories of younger and older adults

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2010)

  • A. Bohn et al.

    The reminiscence bump reconsidered: Children's prospective life stories show a bump in young adulthood

    Psychological Science

    (2011)

  • A. Bohn et al.

    The future is bright and predictable: The development of prospective life stories across childhood and adolescence

    Developmental Psychology

    (2013)

  • G. Cohen et al.

    Life span changes in autobiographical memory

  • K.A. Collins et al.

    Cultural life scripts guide recall of intensely positive life events

    Memory & Cognition

    (2007)

  • M.A. Conway

    A structural model of autobiographical memory

  • M.A. Conway et al.

    Overshadowing the reminiscence bump: Memories of a struggle for independence

    Journal of Adult Development

    (1999)

  • M.A. Conway et al.

    Psychosocial stages and the accessibility of autobiographical memories across the life cycle

    Journal of Personality

    (2004)

  • M.A. Conway et al.

    The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system

    Psychological Review

    (2000)

  • M.A. Conway et al.

    The self and autobiographical memory: Correspondence and coherence

    Social Cognition

    (2004)

  • M.A. Conway et al.

    A cross-cultural investigation of autobiographical memory: On the universality and cultural variation of the reminiscence bump

    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

    (2005)

  • H.F. Crovitz et al.

    Frequency of episodic memories as a function of their age

    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society

    (1974)

  • C. Cuervo-Lombard et al.

    Autobiographical memory of adolescence and early adulthood events: An investigation in schizophrenia

    Journal of the International Neuropsychology Society

    (2007)

  • B. de Vries et al.

    A lifetime of events: Age and gender variations in the life story

    International Journal of Aging & Human Development

    (1996)

  • B. Demiray et al.

    Examining the life story account of the reminiscence bump: Why we remember more from young adulthood

    Memory

    (2009)

  • R.A. Dickson et al.

    The reminiscence bump for salient personal memories: Is a cultural life script required?

    Memory & Cognition

    (2011)

  • H.E. Ebbinghaus

    Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology

    (1964)

  • A.B. Elnick et al.

    Benchmark memories in adulthood: Central domains and predictors of their frequency

    Journal of Adult Development

    (1999)

  • A. Erdoğan et al.

    On the persistence of positive events in life scripts

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2008)

  • E. Erikson

    Childhood and society

    (1950)

  • M.W. Eysenck et al.

    Cognitive psychology: A student's handbook

    (2010)

  • J.M. Fitzgerald

    Vivid memories and the reminiscence phenomenon: The role of a self narrative

    Human Development

    (1988)

  • J.M. Fitzgerald

    The distribution of self-narrative memories in younger and older adults: Elaborating the self-narrative hypothesis

    Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition

    (1996)

  • J.M. Fitzgerald et al.

    Autobiographical memory across the lifespan

    Journal of Gerontology

    (1984)

  • J.M. Fizgerald

    Autobiographical memory and conceptualizations of the self

  • H.C. Franklin et al.

    Personal memories at different ages

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1977)

  • P. Fromholt et al.

    Autobiographical memory in normal aging and primary degenerative dementia (dementia of the Alzheimer type)

    Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences

    (1991)

  • P. Fromholt et al.

    Life-narrative and word-cued autobiographical memories in centenarians: Comparisons with 80-year-old control, depressed, and dementia groups

    Memory

    (2003)

  • F. Galton

    Psychometric experiments

    Brain

    (1879)

  • J. Glück et al.

    Looking back across the life span: A life story account of the reminiscence bump

    Memory & Cognition

    (2007)

  • Navigate DownView more references

    Cited by (74)

    • Autobiographical memory loss in Alzheimer's disease: The role of the reminiscence bump

      2022, Cortex

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Research on autobiographical memory loss in Alzheimer's disease (AD) is characterized by conflicting findings concerning a possible sparing of older memories. The literature shows evidence for both a negative temporal gradient, a flat gradient and a reminiscence bump – that is, a disproportionally high frequency of memories from early adulthood relative to surrounding periods. Here, we expanded the number of lifetime periods of the Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI; Kopelman, Wilson & Baddeley, 1989, 1990) from the standard three to seven in order to increase the sensitivity of the test to variations in the temporal distribution of autobiographical memories across the life span. Twenty-five older adults diagnosed with AD (MMMSE = 21.16, SD = 5.08) and a matched sample of 30 healthy, older adults were assessed. The temporal distribution for personal semantic information in AD showed a temporal gradient steadily decreasing from middle childhood to present life, consistent with predictions derived from consolidation theories. In comparison, the temporal distribution of incidents/episodic memories produced by AD patients in response to the expanded AMI showed a predominance of autobiographical memories from age 6 to 30, followed by a steep drop in memory referring to events that had occurred after age 30. This distribution challenges standard theories of retrograde amnesia in AD by showing neither a temporal gradient, decreasing progressively from early to later life, nor a flat gradient. In contrast, the distribution is consistent with the reminiscence bump identified in autobiographical memory research. Schematization and retrieval support provided by cultural life scripts are discussed.

    • Narrative identity in the psychosis spectrum: A systematic review and developmental model

      2021, Clinical Psychology Review

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Individuals with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders face profound challenges as they attempt to maintain identity through the course of illness. Narrative identity—the study of internalized, evolving life stories—provides a rich theoretical and empirical perspective on these challenges. Based on evidence from a systematic review of narrative identity in the psychosis spectrum (30 studies, combined N = 3859), we argue that the narrative identities of individuals with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders are distinguished by three features: disjointed structure, a focus on suffering, and detached narration. Psychotic disorders typically begin to emerge during adolescence and emerging adulthood, which are formative developmental stages for narrative identity, so it is particularly informative to understand identity disturbances from a developmental perspective. We propose a developmental model in which a focus on suffering emerges in childhood; disjointed structure emerges in middle and late adolescence; and detached narration emerges before or around the time of a first psychotic episode. Further research with imminent risk and early course psychosis populations would be needed to test these predictions. The disrupted life stories of individuals on the psychosis spectrum provide multiple rich avenues for further research to understand narrative self-disturbances.

    • The possible effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on the contents and organization of autobiographical memory: A Transition-Theory perspective

      2021, Cognition

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The COVID-19 Pandemic is unique in its near universal scope and in the way that it has changed our lives. These facts suggest that it might also be unique in its effects on memory. A framework outlined in this article, Transition Theory, is used to explicate the mnemonically relevant ways in which the onset of the Pandemic differs from other personal and collective transitions and how the Pandemic Period might differ from other personally-defined and historically-defined autobiographical periods. Transition Theory also provides the basis for several predictions. Specifically, it predicts (a) a COVID bump (an increase in availability of event memories at the outset of the Pandemic) followed by (b) a lockdown dip (a decrease in availability of event memories from lockdown periods compared to other stable periods). It also predicts that (c) people may consider the Pandemic an important chapter in their life stories, but only when there is little continuity between their pre-Pandemic and post-Pandemic lives. Time will tell whether these predictions pan out. However, it is not too soon to highlight those aspects of the COVID-19 Pandemic that are likely to shape our personal and collective memories of this very unusual historical period.

    • I can still see, hear and smell the fire: Cognitive, emotional and personal consequences of a natural disaster, and the impact of evacuation

      2021, Journal of Environmental Psychology

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      We investigated the effects of evacuation experience on autobiographical memory, sensory-perceptual re-experiencing, emotions, and personal consequentiality of a natural disaster one year after. A total of 601 individuals participated, living nearby the area of the largest fire in modern times in Sweden. It was shown that evacuated (first-hand experience) compared to not-evacuated (second-hand experience) participants thought and talked more about the fire. Evacuated residents also mentally traveled back and re-lived the disaster more; as well as saw the fire, heard its sound, smelled it more, and felt more anxious, enraged, and emotionally strong. Moreover, evacuated compared to not-evacuated participants estimated that their life and view of the world had changed due to the natural disaster. All this suggests that the psychology of dramatically charged events, such as natural disasters, differs notably between individuals “being there” and those “hearing the news”, indicating a factual flashbulb memory as a result of the first-hand experience.

    • Not “WEIRD” but Truly Different: Cultural Life Scripts and Autobiographical Memory in Indigenous Australia

      2021, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      This exploratory study examines the structure of cultural life scripts (CLS) and autobiographical memories in a group of 13 non-WEIRD (Western Educated Industrialized Rich Democratic) Nunggubuyu from Australia. Participants provided a CLS and reported the events’ expected timing and valence; and their seven most important memories, the age at the events and their valence. The CLS and important memories consisted mainly of positive events happening in early youth, but there was also a notable bump with positive events later in life. Results provide evidence for the important role of grandmothers in the Nunggubuyu culture, as both in the CLSs and in autobiographical memories, many events related to learning about Nunggubuyu culture as a child, and grandmothers teaching the community and especially children. Importantly, the results highlight that societal and cultural structures that differ from those in WEIRD communities may result in differences in CLS and in autobiographical memory recall.

    • Academic Forgetting

      2020, Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition

    Arrow Up and RightView all citing articles on Scopus

    • Research article

      Commentary on Koppel and Berntsen: How many reminiscence bumps are there?

      Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Volume 4, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 81-83

    • Research article

      Similarity to the self affects memory for impressions of others

      Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Volume 4, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 20-28

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      The present studies investigated whether similarity to the self influenced memory for impressions of others. We predicted that similarity to the self would facilitate impression memory for others, paralleling the self-reference effect found when information is processed relative to the self. We were interested in how the initial valence of the impression, whether positive or negative, affected impression memory. Across two experiments, participants formed impressions while viewing faces paired with traits and behaviors. After recognition, participants rated the self-descriptiveness of the studied traits allowing impression memory to be sorted into high-, medium-, and low-self-similarity. For positive impressions, similar others were remembered better than dissimilar others. For negative impressions, similar others were remembered more poorly than dissimilar others. These results illustrate that similarity to the self has multifaceted effects on person memory, leading to memory enhancement in the case of people given positive impressions, but reducing memory for people associated with negative impressions.

    • Research article

      Memory integration in amnesia: Prior knowledge supports verbal short-term memory

      Neuropsychologia, Volume 70, 2015, pp. 272-280

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      Short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM) have traditionally been considered cognitively distinct. However, it is known that STM can improve when to-be-remembered information appears in contexts that make contact with prior knowledge, suggesting a more interactive relationship between STM and LTM. The current study investigated whether the ability to leverage LTM in support of STM critically depends on the integrity of the hippocampus. Specifically, we investigated whether the hippocampus differentially supports between-domain versus within-domain STM–LTM integration given prior evidence that the representational domain of the elements being integrated in memory is a critical determinant of whether memory performance depends on the hippocampus. In Experiment 1, we investigated hippocampal contributions to within-domain STM–LTM integration by testing whether immediate verbal recall of words improves in MTL amnesic patients when words are presented in familiar verbal contexts (meaningful sentences) compared to unfamiliar verbal contexts (random word lists). Patients demonstrated a robust sentence superiority effect, whereby verbal STM performance improved in familiar compared to unfamiliar verbal contexts, and the magnitude of this effect did not differ from that in controls. In Experiment 2, we investigated hippocampal contributions to between-domain STM–LTM integration by testing whether immediate verbal recall of digits improves in MTL amnesic patients when digits are presented in a familiar visuospatial context (a typical keypad layout) compared to an unfamiliar visuospatial context (a random keypad layout). Immediate verbal recall improved in both patients and controls when digits were presented in the familiar compared to the unfamiliar keypad array, indicating a preserved ability to integrate activated verbal information with stored visuospatial knowledge. Together, these results demonstrate that immediate verbal recall in amnesia can benefit from two distinct types of semantic support, verbal and visuospatial, and that the hippocampus is not critical for leveraging stored semantic knowledge to improve memory performance.

    • Research article

      Why does the adolescence bump differ from the emergent adulthood bump in autobiographical memories?

      Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Volume 4, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 84-86

    • Research article

      One bump, two bumps, three bumps, four? Using retrieval cues to divide one autobiographical memory reminiscence bump into many

      Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, Volume 4, Issue 1, 2015, pp. 87-89

    • Research article

      The mood-enhancement function of autobiographical memories: Comparisons with other functions in terms of emotional valence

      Consciousness and Cognition, Volume 70, 2019, pp. 88-100

      Show abstractNavigate Down

      In two studies, we examined the emotional valence of memories used for mood-enhancement in relation to memories serving self, social and directive functions. Our sample included a total of 263 participants aged between 45 and 82 years. In Study 1, participants recalled memories in response to 51 cue words. In Study 2, participants recalled 32 memories that served the four functions (eight memories per function). We used multilevel modeling to take into consideration the hierarchical nature of our datasets (memories nested within individuals). Study 1 showed that emotional valence was positively associated with mood-enhancement and social functions, whereas negatively related to self and directive functions. This relation was strongest for the mood-enhancement function. In Study 2, mood-enhancing memories were rated as more positive than self, social and directive memories. We discussed results in terms of the tripartite model of memory functions and proposed that mood-enhancement should represent a distinct function.

    View full text

    Copyright © 2014 Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    What does the reminiscence bump refer to?

    The reminiscence bump is the increased proportion of autobiographical memories from youth and early adulthood observed in adults over 40. It is one of the most robust findings in autobiographical memory research.

    What causes the reminiscence bump?

    The reminiscence bump is caused by age-related differences in encoding efficiency, which cause more memories to be stored in adolescence and early adulthood.

    At what stage of life does the reminiscence bump occur quizlet?

    The reminiscence bump refers to the phenomenon of older adults having the strongest and most vivid memories for events that happened between the ages of 10 and 30.

    What is the youth bias and which explanation of the reminiscence bump is it associated with?

    what is the youth bias, and which explanation of the reminiscence bump is it associated with? Tendency for the most notable public events in a person's life to be perceived to occur when the person is young.