Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

2Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health, Babeş-Bolyai University of Cluj-Napoca, Romania

3Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, NewYork

Find articles by Daniel David

Disclaimer

1Doctoral School Evidence-Based Assesment and Psychological Interventions, Babes-Bolyai University

2Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, The International Institute for the Advanced Studies of Psychotherapy and Applied Mental Health, Babeş-Bolyai University of Cluj-Napoca, Romania

3Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, NewYork

Alina Cimpean, Doctoral School Evidence-Based Assesment and Psychological Interventions, Republicii Street, no. 37, Cluj-Napoca, 400273, Romania. Email: [email protected]

Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).

Abstract

This study emphasizes the role of pain catastrophizing and state anxiety on pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. Response expectancies for pain tolerance and pain intensity were tested as potential mediators. A cold-pressor task was used on a healthy sample. Measures were taken before and after cold-pressor procedure, while aversive versus neutral information regarding the cold-pressor task was given prior to the pain induction. An exploratory path model was conducted. Similar paths for pain tolerance and for pain-related anxiety were shown in aversive condition. Also, psychological measures may explain pain experience in aversive context.

Keywords: cognitive behavioral therapy, cold-pressor task, pain-related anxiety, pain tolerance

Introduction

The main dimensions of the sensorial component of pain are pain tolerance and pain intensity (; ; ). Pain tolerance is defined as the maximum amount of pain that a person can bear (). Literature shows that lower levels of pain tolerance are often associated with depression, fear of future disability, frustration, anger, lower activity levels, reduced pleasure, isolation, disruption of intimate relationships, a sense of helplessness, and addiction to medication (). Increased pain tolerance is an important resilience factor, which helps patients to alleviate the suffering that often comes with pain (; ).

As literature suggests, since pain cannot be avoided, it is highly important to investigate it in terms of tolerance (). In the cognitive behavioral framework, the ability to tolerate discomfort has been defined as frustration tolerance (; ). also refers to this as nonawfulizing about badness (). A high level of frustration tolerance is considered a determiner of resilience, while low levels of frustration tolerance and high levels of awfulizing/catastrophizing are predictive of large areas of psychopathology ().

Pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety are two of the most studied outcomes in patients who suffer from pain. Pain-related anxiety is one of the most frequent emotions associated with pain (). It has been claimed that the feelings of anxiety become such a fundamental part of pain experience that patients may have difficulty differentiating between the two (; ). Given the impact of both pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety on patient’s well-being, physical status, and mental health (), research has investigated the cognitive and emotional predictors underlying these constructs. Pain catastrophizing, anxiety, and response expectancies are three of the most studied variables (; ).

Pain catastrophizing, anxiety, response expectancies, and pain intensity

Pain catastrophizing is defined as a maladaptative cognition, comprising three different dimensions (magnification, rumination, helplessness), measured by Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS; ). Considerable research has shown that pain catastrophizing is a significant determiner of pain experience and pain-related outcomes (). Relationships between pain catastrophizing, pain intensity, and pain tolerance have been shown in several clinical populations. It is associated with decreased pain tolerance and higher pain severity among patients with rheumatoid arthritis, with more severe and widespread pain, with higher levels of emotional distress among individuals with fibromyalgia and scleroderma, and with disability up to 6 months postoperatively in patients who have had knee surgery for osteoarthritis (see ).

Also, anxiety is associated with pain intenity (), pain tolerance as a response to pain intensification, prolonged elevation of pain levels, or anticipation of increased pain (; ). Studies on clinical population suggest that preoperative psychological distress has a significant impact on postsurgical recovery, and it is associated with poor clinical outcomes (such as: high levels of pain intensity and emotional distress) after surgery to the hip, knee, and lumbar spine (; ). Specifically, higher levels of preoperative state anxiety are associated with increased postoperative pain intensity (; ), which may significantly influence pain tolerance (). It is generally accepted that pain catastrophizing and anxiety are theoretically distinct from one another (; ). Also, there is evidence to suggest that the two, although associated, have distinct roles in pain. For example, pain catastrophizing has been shown to predict the unique variance in functional disability over trait anxiety in a community sample of children () and the variance in pain and disability over negative affectivity in a small clinical sample of youths with chronic pain ().

Given their impact on pain intensity, response expectancies emerged as a predictor of pain-related outcomes, with an important contribution in the understanding of pain experience (). It has been claimed that nonvolitional responses, including pain reactions, are impacted by response expectancies (; ). They were defined as the expectation that a nonvolitional response will occur, as a function of a behavior or a specific stimulus (; ). More specifically, response expectancies regarding the appearance of a nonvolitional response are sufficient to create nonvolitional outcomes, such as memory reports, pain perception, responses to psychotherapy, sexual arousal, asthmatic responses, and mood (). They are not mediated by other psychological variables (; ). For example, in case of placebo effects, the effects of hypnotic suggestion, and the effects of pharmacological agents and medical interventions were not identified any psychological mechanisms to explain their impact on behavior (; ). Moreover, response expectancies are self-confirming and might be influenced by verbal instructions, such as the informations regarding the effect that a procedure is expected to have (; ).

Moreover, response expectancies are self-confirming, might be influenced by verbal instructions such as the informations regarding the effect that a procedure is expected to have (; ). Also, the expected pain level is correlated with the experienced pain level (). In chronic pain, response expectancies and pain catastrophizing influence the large individual differences that can be observed in the covariation of daily mood and daily pain (; ; ). Specifically, response expectancies for pain tolerance are predictive of pain intensity and the level of pain tolerance (; ; ). Thus, response expectancies might potentially explain the relationships between pain catastrophizing/state anxiety and pain tolerance/pain-related anxiety, although little is known about how they influence these relationships in aversive contexts.

Aversive versus nonaversive pain contexts

The context of a noxious stimulus is highly relevant to pain-related outcomes as it may activate dysfunctional beliefs (; ), such as pain catastrophizing. According to the literature, dysfunctional beliefs need a cue to become manifest (), and once activated, they may have a significant impact on behavioral and emotional responses (; ). Nevertheless, aversive pain contexts are more likely to activate cognitive vulnerabilities (); (see Figure 1 and Figure 2).

Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

Open in a separate window

Figure 1.

Graphic representation of paths predicting pain tolerance in aversive/neutral condition.

Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

Open in a separate window

Figure 2.

Graphic representation of paths predicting pain-related anxiety in aversive/neutral condition.

The path analytic model

The impact of pain catastrophizing and anxiety on response expectancies may be explained by the history of heightened pain experience, in different contexts, of individuals who catastrophize (). These patients may develop expectancies of future pain experiences or may develop specific beliefs regarding the aversiveness associated with pain related contexts (; ). Therefore, prior experience with pain, or associated beliefs about painfull situations of these patients may lead to expectancies that aversive contexts will bring a high degree of pain, which may impact pain tolerance () and distress (). Therefore, whether on the basis of prior painful experience, or associated beliefs about pain-eliciting situations, individuals who catastrophize may expect that future pain-eliciting situations will be associated with a high degree of pain, which may impact pain tolerance () and distress (). Also, literature suggests that response expectancies would predict pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety via pain intensity. The research shows that response expectancies are very good predictors of both positive (relaxation) and negative (e.g. pain intensity or distress) nonvolitional outcomes in clinical and nonclinical samples (). The role of response expectancies as a psychological mechanism for producing nonvolitional outcomes, such as pain perception, memory reports, responses to psychotherapy, sexual arousal, asthmatic responses, and mood is known in the literature (see ; ; ).

Because response expectancies tend to be self-confirming (), as it is amply documented by the vast literature on placebo effects, response expectancies impact pain intensity, while pain intensity influence pain tolerance. Pain tolerance may be influenced by targeting response expectancies for pain tolerance and pain intensity level. Similarly, placebos reduce self-reported pain intensity and enhance pain tolerance (; ).

We hope that by expanding the pathways that explain pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety, and by exploring possible common predictors of both sensorial and affective dimensions of pain, to facilitate a better understanding of the processes involved in pain and emotion. As literature indicated (; ), investigating the ways in which cognitive and emotional factors interact in specific conditions might increase the effectiveness of interventions for pain management.

The transdiagnostic model

Since pain and comorbid emotional distress appear to be inseparable processes, a transdiagnostic perspective would facilitate the flexibility in delivery of psychotherapeutic interventions (). Psychological interventions are useful for patients with acute pain, who follow painful medical preocedures, or for patients who developed chronic pain (). As literature suggests, a painful event might activate specific beliefs about pain and threat, which activate negative dysfunctional emotions (; ), with a negative impact on recovery (see ). For example, patients with depression are three times less likely than nondepressed patients to adhere to treatment recommendations (). Also, patients who reported depression symptoms reported significantly more pain (), while the presence of persistent pain significantly increases the risk of future depression, major depression or anxiety, and vice versa (). found that the prevalence of major depression in patients with pain ranged between 13 percent and 85 percent. Likewise, people with chronic pain frequently report significant expressions of fear and anxiety in both community and treatment-seeking samples (). Conversely, the prevalence of pain among patients with major depression ranged between 15 percent and 100 percent (). The relation between pain and emotion is bidirectional (). Therefore, hospitalized patients who follow painful procedures could benefit the most from interventions addressed to the management of dysfunctional emotions and pain, preventing them from developing chronic pain, disability, or psychopathology ().

As previously mentioned, the transdiagnostic approach assume that certain psychological processes contribute in a causal way to the development and maintenance of symptoms and suggest that they are underpinned by common psychological mechanisms (). From this perspective, it would be useful to identify the adaptive strategies and to facilitate the integration of psychotherapy into diverse health care settings (). In addition, the transdiagnostic model for pain and emotion may facilitate crossover treatments, namely treatments whose efficacy has been demonstrated for more than one of a cluster of component symptoms, and which may be beneficial in treating the symptom cluster as a whole (). Where there is a broad spectrum of effects on other symptoms in the cluster, noted that (1) the symptoms may share a common etiology, (2) diminishing one symptom may prevent exacerbation of the others, and (3) single interventions may be indicated for more than one symptom. As noted, the generation of “crossover” treatments has the possible benefit of using a single intervention to simplify treatment, which may reduce costs.

Overview of the present study

The impact of anxiety and pain catasatrophizing has been investigated in separate models, without considering the shared variance (; ). Therefore, we need to clarify the interconnected relations between the variables using a single model for each outcome of interest, investigating whether pain catastophizing and anxiety are statistically distinct, or whether they are uniquely related to pain-related outcomes (see ). Moreover, the relationships between pain catastrophizing/state anxiety and pain tolerance/pain-related anxiety have been extensively investigated in the literature, but the mechanisms behind these associations remain poorly understood.

Therefore, the present study aims to expand on the pathways that explain pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety by exploring pain catastrophizing and state anxiety as possible predictors. Drawing on the previous findings, response expectancies for pain tolerance and their impact on pain intensity were investigated as potential mechanisms. By identifying the paths that may explain both pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety, we will take a step toward a transdiagnostic perspective for pain and anxiety (). Given the need for a threatening activator for dysfunctional cognitions (; ), we manipulated the threat value of the pain associated with cold-pressor task using verbal information about its potential consequences. We expected that participants in aversive condition would report higher levels of state anxiety than participants included in neutral condition. We also expected that in aversive condition, pain catastrophizing and state anxiety would indirectly predict pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety via response expectancies. In addition, we expected that response expectancies for pain tolerance would indirectly predict pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety via pain intensity.

Finally, the assessment of distorted cognitions in aversive contexts is a major concern of the literature. Research highlights that state measures of pain catastrophizing are more valid than trait measures, which may not adequately capture the variance in reported pain (). Literature also suggests that aversive pain contexts represent one possible solution for highlighting state pain catastrophizing in experimental studies (), thereby increasing the generalizability of results (see ).

Methods

Participants

In total, 78 undergraduate students were included in the study. The participants ranged in age from 19 to 33 years (mean = 21.74). Individuals suffering from medical conditions associated with persistent pain (such as migraine, headache, and back pain) and those with other conditions that might be adversely affected by pain procedure (e. g. cardiovascular problems, previous episodes of frostbite, etc.) were not considered for participation. All participants signed a written informed consent.

Procedure

Experimental manipulation of anxiety

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two experimental conditions: (1) the aversive condition (n = 41) and (2) the neutral condition (n = 37). Written information regarding the task was given. Participants were told that important information about the task will be given, and they were told to read carefully before signing the informed consent. Participants from the aversive condition received written information which informed them that in some extreme cases, the cold-pressor task could be dangerous and might result in a serious degeneration of the immersed hand, while those in the neutral condition were told that the task was very similar to searching for a drink in a freezer and that it would not result in any physical injury. After signing the informed consent, participants were then instructed to place their hand into the water and to keep it immersed for as long as they could tolerate. After 5 minutes, participants were told to extract their hand. The data were screened for missing values and outliers, as these can have a significant impact on path analyses. For both experimental conditions, the path analytic model was tested separately for each of the main outcome in the study, namely pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety.

Measures

Pain catastrophizing

The PCS ( ) is a 13-item self-report measure of pain catastrophizing that assesses the negative thoughts associated with pain. The PCS measures three dimensions of catastrophizing: rumination, magnification, and helplessness. Participants rate their feelings of pain on a five-point Likert-type scale (where 0 is “not at all,” and 4 is “all the time”). PCS scores range from 0 to 52, where higher scores indicate more pain catastrophizing. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alphas were as follows: full scale, α = .87; helplessness, α = .78; magnification, α = .63; rumination, α = .82.

State anxiety

The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; ) is a commonly used measure of trait and state anxiety. The most popular version has 20 items for assessing trait anxiety and 20 for state anxiety. We used the items from state anxiety subscale. Higher scores indicate greater anxiety. Internal consistency coefficients for the scale range from .86 to .95; test–retest reliability coefficients over a 2-month interval range from .65 to .75 (). Considerable evidence attests to the construct and concurrent validity of the scale (). Studies have also shown that scores can vary with changes in health and other individual characteristics ().

Response expectancies for pain tolerance

Participants were asked to rate on a visual analogue scale (VAS), the level of pain tolerance they expected to have during the cold-pressor task by marking a line between 0 (no pain tolerance) and 100 (extreme pain tolerance).

Example of item: Please rate how much pain tolerance you are expecting to have during cold-pressor test.

Pain-related anxiety

Participants were given a VAS scale for completion after cold-pressor procedure. Reports of the anxiety they experienced were collected by marking a line between 0 (no anxiety) and 100 (extreme anxiety).

Pain measures

Pain tolerance was the total time, in seconds, that the participant’s hand was immersed in the water, minus pain threshold time.

Pain threshold was determined by asking participants to report the moment when they began to feel pain or discomfort. The time, in seconds, between the start of the immersion and the reporting of the pain was recorded as the pain threshold.

Pain intensity was the pain experienced during the cold-pressor task, measured using a VAS, which ranged from 0 (no pain) to 100 (extreme pain). Participants were asked to mark a line between 0 and 100 to suggest the intensity of pain. Example of item: Please rate the level of pain intensity you experienced during experimental procedure.

Apparatus

A cold-pressor apparatus was used to induce pain. The apparatus consisted of a refrigeration unit that cooled constantly circulating water in an insulated container measuring 30 cm × 40 cm × 30 cm. The water temperature was maintained at 5°C. Water at room temperature (21°C) was used to standardize the temperature of the hand before immersion in the cold water container. The apparatus was a Refrigerated Bath Circulator, Model JSRC-13C.

Statistical analyses

Student’s t-test was used to evaluate whether the experimental manipulation influenced state anxiety before pain induction. Path analysis was used to identify the model that was the most predictive for pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. We found one model that fit the data well. This model was tested for each experimental condition, considering pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety as separate outcomes. This statistical method is an extension of multiple regression. Unlike multiple regression, which focusses on the prediction of a single dependent variable, path analysis provide the posibility to investigate more than one dependent variable (). It is a usefull statisical method in the research of bio-psycho-social relationships because it assess multiple relations within a model, offering a theoretical, directional, and a predictive model of those relationships, (). It presents a theoretical, directional relationship (both direct and indirect) between variables, and as such, offers a predictive model of those relationships (). In this study, the hypothesized model was tested in an exploratory manner, based on the contributing variables, namely pain tolerance and anxiety. Following the recommendations of , model-data fit was examined using several fit indices, including the comparative fit index (CFI), the nonnormed fit index or Tucker–Lewis index (TLI), the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). Statistical significance of the path coefficients was also analyzed. We used R () and Lavaan Package () to perform these analyses.

Results

Descriptive analyses

We had 41 participants in the aversive condition and 37 participants in the neutral condition. They were undergraduate psychology students, in the second and third year of study. Means and standard deviations were calculated for each measure (see Table 1).

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics.

ConditionMeasuresNMSDAversive groupPain catastrophizing4120.4612.36State anxiety4136.8618.74Response expectancies for pain tolerance4133.720.07Pain intensity4141.9527.2Pain tolerance4162.513.25Pain-related anxiety4141.8317.82Neutral groupPain catastrophizing3719.9413.41State anxiety3725.4217.72Response expectancies for pain tolerance3728.1318.54Pain intensity3734.5626.4Pain tolerance37118.6823.2Pain-related anxiety3740.4212.31

Open in a separate window

SD: standard deviation.

Manipulation check

Our results showed that the experimental manipulation was effective. There were significant differences between conditions on state anxiety level (t = 2.04, p < .05; see Table 2).

Table 2.

Manipulation check.

Aversive groupNeutral groupt-testpMmState anxiety36.8625.422.04.004

Open in a separate window

The path model for predicting pain tolerance

Various fit indices were used to assess the adequacy of the path models. The goodness-of-fit index (GFI) of 0.993 indicated an excellent fit. The root mean square residual (RMSEA) of 0.024 was within the expected range of unaccounted variance (<0.05) and represented a high level of closeness of fit for the model. The Tucker–Lewis coefficient and CFI were 1 for both aversive and neutral conditions, also indicating a good fit.

Direct and indirect paths for pain tolerance: aversive vs. neutral condition

The first path analysis model was constructed to investigate whether pain catastrophizing and state anxiety are indirect predictors of pain tolerance. For the aversive condition, with the exception of one path, all the estimated direct paths were statistically significant. Pain catastrophizing had a direct effect on response expectancies (β = 3.29, p < .005). State anxiety had a direct effect on response expectancies (β = −2.81, p < .001). Response expectancies had a direct effect on pain intensity (β = .78, p < .000). Pain intensity did not predict pain tolerance (β = −2.86, p < .68). Response expectancies had a direct effect on pain tolerance (β = 2.90, p < .009). Pain catastrophizing did not predict pain tolerance via response expectancies (β = −.25, p < .57). In contrast, state anxiety significantly predicted pain tolerance via response expectancies (β = −0.91, p < .003). A significant indirect path was also found from response expectancies to pain tolerance via pain intensity (β = −2.25, p < .004).

For the neutral condition, the only significant paths were from state anxiety to response expectancies (β = −1.05, p < .003) and from response expectancies to pain intensity (β = .71, p < .000; see Figures 3 and and4;4; Table 3).

Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

Open in a separate window

Figure 3.

Graphic representation of paths predicting pain tolerance in neutral condition.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

Open in a separate window

Figure 4.

Graphic representation of paths predicting pain tolerance in aversive condition.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Table 3.

Direct and indirect paths for predicting pain tolerance.

Aversive conditionNeutral conditionEstimate 95%CIpEstimate 95%CIpDirect effectsPain catastrophizing - Response expectancies3,290,005−0,620,377State anxiety - Response expectancies−2,810,001−1,05−0,032Response expectancies - Pain intensity0,7800,710Pain Intensity - Pain tolerance−2,860,680,150,731Response expectancies - Pain tolerance2,90,009−0,530,275Indirect effectsPain Catastrophizing - Response Expectancies - Pain tolerance−0,250,57−0,030,742State anxiety - Response expectancies - Pain tolerance−0,910,0030,040,735Response Expectancies - Pain Intensity - Pain tolerance−2,250,0040,110,731

Open in a separate window

CI: confidence interval.

Various fit indices were used to assess the adequacy of the path model for the aversive and the neutral condition. For aversive condition, the GFI was 0.983, indicating an excellent fit. The RMSEA was 0.024, the Tucker–Lewis coefficient was 0.93, and the CFI was 0.98, also indicating a good fit. In the case of the neutral condition, the GFI was 0.951, RMSEA was 0.000, indicating a good fit, while the TLC was 0.27 and the CFI was 0.71, which indicates a medium to low fit with the model.

We analyzed whether pain catastrophizing and state anxiety were indirect predictors of pain-related anxiety. For the aversive condition, with the exception of one path, all the estimated directs paths were statistically significant. Pain catastrophizing had a direct effect on response expectancies (β = .36, p < .006). State anxiety had a direct effect on response expectancies (β = .26, p < .001). Response expectancies had a direct effect on pain intensity (β = 1.13, p < .000). Pain intensity predicted pain-related anxiety (β = −.28, p < .000). Response expectancies did not predict pain-related anxiety (β = −.004, p < .980).

Pain catastrophizing was not a significant predictor of pain-related anxiety via response expectancies (β = −.02, p < .708). In contrast, a significant indirect path was found from state anxiety to pain-related anxiety via response expectancies (β = −.16, p < .003). A significant indirect path was also found from response expectancies to pain-related anxiety via pain intensity (β = −.32, p < .009).

For the neutral condition, the paths from pain catastrophizing to response expectancies and from response expectancies to pain-related anxiety were insignificant. The direct path from state anxiety to response expectancies was significant (β = .19, p < .002). The direct path from response expectancies to pain intensity was significant (β = .76, p < .000). The path from pain intensity to pain-related anxiety was also significant (β = −.26, p < .000), while the only significant indirect path was from response expectancies to pain-related anxiety, via pain intensity (β = −.20, p < .005; see Figures 5 and and6;6; see Table 4).

Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

Open in a separate window

Figure 5.

Graphic representation of paths predicting pain-related anxiety in neutral condition.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Pain tolerance is influenced by expectancies and interpretation of experiences

Open in a separate window

Figure 6.

Graphic representation of paths predicting pain-related anxiety in aversive condition.

*p < .05; **p < .01.

Table 4.

Direct and indirect paths for predicting pain-related anxiety.

Aversive conditionNeutral conditionEstimate 95%CIpEstimate 95%CIpDirect effectsPain catastrophizing - Response expectancies0,360,0060,120,34State anxiety - Response expectancies0,260,010,190,02Response expectancies - Pain intensity1,1300,760Pain Intensity - Pain-related anxiety−0,280,005−0,260,02Response expectancies - Pain-related anxiety−0,0040,980,040,655Indirect effectsPain Catastrophizing - Response Expectancies - Pain-related anxiety−0,020,7080,120,11State anxiety - Response expectancies - Pain-related anxiety−0,160,03−0,080,12Response Expectancies - Pain Intensity - Pain-related anxiety−0,350,009−0,20,005

Open in a separate window

CI: confidence interval.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether pain catastrophizing and state anxiety are indirect predictors of pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. Response expectancies for pain tolerance were tested as a mechanism of both pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. We also tested whether response expectancies for pain tolerance indirectly predict pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety, via pain intensity. Two different activating contexts (aversive and neutral) were experimentally induced. The threat value of cold-pressor pain was manipulated using written information about the task and the procedure was performed by healthy/pain-free volunteers. Our results showed that the experimental manipulation was successful: participants in the aversive condition reported significantly higher levels of anxiety prior to the task. The proposed path model, which was based on a priori considerations and past research, fit the data well. Our results demonstrate that aversive contexts have an important influence on pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. Significant paths were found for the aversive condition, separately predicting pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. Our results indicate that these outcomes are influenced by an aversive perception of the task, which activates threat anxiety and distorted cognitions (). The path analytic model shows that for the aversive condition, response expectancies for pain tolerance significantly predict pain intensity and pain tolerance, while in neutral condition they predict only the intensity of the pain. Interestingly, response expectancies were not the mechanism for the relationship between pain catastrophizing and pain tolerance/pain-related anxiety, but instead they mediated the relationship between state anxiety and pain tolerance/pain-related anxiety. Pain intensity also mediated the relationship between response expectancies and pain tolerance/pain-related anxiety, in separate models. These results emphasize the findings described in literature, namely that response expectancies predict nonvolitional responses (; ). Nevertheless, in aversive contexts, response expectancies may also be expressed in the form of behavioral responses, such as pain tolerance (). We have therefore demonstrated that they are relevant mechanisms which may influence the level of perceived pain, pain tolerance, and pain-related anxiety.

In the aversive condition, pain catastrophizing significantly predicts pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety, while in a neutral context it is not predictive. Therefore, as the previous literature suggests, pain catastrophizing might represent a latent construct, requiring sufficient activation in order to exert its effects (). These results confirms that a negative activating event may highlight cognitive vulnerabilities or may emphasize latent maladaptive thoughts in need of a cue to become manifest (). Catastrophic or dysfunctional beliefs need threat, real or inferred, to elicit emotional or behavioral responses (; ). Since context may activate underlying beliefs, the literature suggests that it is strongly relevant to pain-related outcomes (). Once activated, maladaptive beliefs may also have a significant impact on behavioral and emotional responses (). Catastrophizing is one of four irrational beliefs, a main mechanism, and an important target for Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT) interventions in broad area of psychopathology (; ; ; ). Pain catastrophizing have also been treated as an index of change of dysfunctional cognition and represents a therapeutic mechanism by which CBT can reduce pain and improve functioning ().

In our study, state anxiety predicted pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety by the way of response expectancies, with higher levels of anxiety predicting lower response expectancies for pain tolerance. These results replicate previous findings described in the literature, which state that the mechanism through which anxiety perpetuates its effect is based on response expectancies (). As recent findings suggest, these are the core mechanisms of anxiety (), enhanced by avoidance cognitions and dysfunctional behaviors (; ).

For the advance in the field, research suggests that identifying the mechanisms that would afford a greater understanding of both problems would facilitate the progress concerning the theoretical understanding and treatment of pain and emotion (). Also, given the lack of a clear theoretical understanding of the processes involved, the treatment of these comorbid factors needs more research (). Our study showed that cognitive and emotional factors are closely linked with pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety, especially in aversive condition. Also, these results highlight that pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety share common mechanisms when threat is perceived. As previously stated, in order to add data to the transdiagnostic model for pain and emotion, we need to emphasize certain psychological processes contributing in a causal way to the development and maintenance of various symptoms observed across patients (). Therefore, our data may add significant support for the transdiagnostic model of pain and emotion.

Although CBT is a theoretical approach with a powerful empirical support, efficacious either alone or as an adjunct to medication, and highly efficient regarding relapse and recurrence (; ), there is still a strong need to address specific cognitive factors in order be more effective in reducing certain emotional dysfunctionalities (). Thus, literature encourages a better understanding of the involved cognitive factors and mechanisms of change assumed to increase the effectiveness of intervention packages (). Our study investigated possible therapeutic targets underlying the process of patient adaptation to aversive circumstances. From a CBT perspective, they consist in patient cognitions and behaviors (see ). Our study confirms that for patients who suffer from acute pain, thoughts/cognitive processes, emotions, and behaviors are interconnected (). Therefore, adaptive ways of thinking, feeling, and behaving in threatening situations can be achieved to help patients cope with pain and dysfunctional emotions related.

Theoretical and clinical implications

A patient’s personal experience of pain in aversive contexts may be explained by pain catastrophizing, state anxiety, and response expectancies for pain tolerance. The findings of the present study encourage a transdiagnostic perspective for treating pain-related outcomes, such as pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. The transdiagnostic model promotes the transfer of practical and theoretical advances between disorders, facilitates the transfer of research findings to a broader range of disorders, and provides an explanation for the comorbidity observed in clinical practice (). By identifying the common features driving both problems, the transdiagnostic model aims to better understand the procesess involved (). Our study found similar mechanisms of pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety in aversive condition. As previously mentioned, it is relevant to highlight the overlapping features or common maintaining mechanisms in order to support a transdiagnostic view (). Our results also provide more empirical support for the use of the CBT approach with patients who suffer from acute pain by showing the association between cognitive/emotional factors and clinically relevant aspects, and the path through which they affect pain outcomes. Also for these patients, changing dysfunctional evaluations may be considered an important mechanism for changing dysfunctional outcomes (see ; ). Numerous studies have shown pain catastrophizing to be associated with dysfunctional beliefs regarding the ability to cope with the difficulties caused by the experience of pain, with the exaggeration of problems, and with ignoring the positive aspects of the situation, such as the availability of resources (), resulting in fear of pain, depression, and disability ().

Our results also highlight that framing painful events in terms of threat may increase the level of reported anxiety (), while an interaction between threat and distorted cognitions could lead to emotional and behavioral difficulties (). Moreover, such framing may influence an individual’s behavioral decisions and pain tolerance (). It is important to emphasize the effect of information communication in health-threatening situations, since numerous studies indicate that the behavioral and emotional outcomes of the patient are influenced, on one hand, by their underlying cognitions, and on the other hand, by the general framing offered by health care providers (). Nevertheless, screening for the cognitive and emotional predictors of dysfunctional health-related outcomes is a low-cost method by which individuals who might benefit the most from psychological interventions can be identified.

Nevertheless, our study has a significant impact in terms of generalizability to population who suffer from acute pain. As defined by , generalization involves drawing broad conclusions from particular instances, or making an inference about the unobserved based on the observed. The statistical generalization, namely the applicability of research findings beyond the particular people who took part in a study, is met in our study due to the research design and experimental manipulation. Although our results may not be generalized to clinical patients (mainly because of the health status of our sample-healthy population), it may be extrapolated to healthy patients who are following specific threatening procedures (acute pain), since our data showed that the aversiveness of the task was successfully induced and different results followed.

Also, random distribution of participants is another important aspect of generalization. The best strategy for achieving a representative sample is to use probability (random) methods of sampling (). Nevertheless, due to the threat induction through experimental manipulation, our study supports transferability of results to patients who follow painfull medical procedures.

The limitations of the present research have implications for the generalizability of the findings to clinical samples. The participants were undergraduate students who were not suffering from pain-related health conditions. The applicability of the results to patients suffering from a clinical condition or chronic pain is thus uncertain. The second issue is one of measurement. Although using a visual analogue scale to evaluate pain-related anxiety and response expectancies is a common practice, it has several limitations () which may have influenced our results. First limit addressed by is concerning the subject’s ability to conceptually understand the method itself, although the VAS is described as being independent of language. It includes the ability of the subject to conceive the unit of the line as a representation of a personal perception of an abstract concept. Therefore, any mark along the line between these two extremes is totally dependent upon the subject’s unique interpretation (). The second limit underlined by the authors mentioned above is based on the inaccuracy of reproduction of the instrument due to the distortion that often appear on Xeroxing, known to distort the length of the line. In addition, there may appear some effects eye-hand coordination (). Furthermore, we used only observational and self-report data, which may result in under-reporting of the main outcomes. Future studies should also focus on physiological measures of the sensorial characteristics of pain and pain-related anxiety in order to gather more complex and valid information ().

Conclusion

As literature indicated (; ; ), there is a strong need to increase the effectiveness of interventions for pain management. Our study identified mutual factors driving both problems, which may afford expanding the pathways that explain pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety. By exploring possible common predictors of both sensorial and affective dimensions of pain, we ultimately aimed to facilitate a better understanding of the link between pain and emotion, as well as improving knowledge of the processes involved. In aversive condition, pain catastrophizing significantly predicts pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety, while in a neutral context it is not predictive. Therefore, as the previous literature suggests, pain catastrophizing might represent a latent construct, requiring sufficient activation in order to exert its effects (see ; ; ). Nevertheless, our study may add significant input for populations who are following aversive procedures and acute pain, emphasizing the mechanisms involved in pain tolerance and pain-related anxiety.

Footnotes

Declaration of conflicting interests: The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship and/or publication of this article.

Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

  • Asmundson GJ, Abrams MP, Collimore KC. (2008) Pain and anxiety disorders. In: Zvolensky MJ, Smits JAJ. (eds) Anxiety in Health Behaviors and Physical Illness. New York: Springer, pp. 207–235. [Google Scholar]
  • Asmundson GJ, Katz J. (2009) Understanding the co-occurrence of anxiety disorders and chronic pain: State-of-the-art. Depression and Anxiety 26(10): 888–901. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bair MJ, Robison RL, Katon W, et al. (2003) Depression and pain comorbidity: A literature review. Archives of Internal Medicine 163(20): 2433–2445. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baker SL, Kirsch I. (1991) Cognitive mediators of pain perception and tolerance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 61(3): 504–510. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Beck AT, Dozois DJA. (2011) Cognitive therapy: Current status and future directions. Annual Review of Medicine 62(1): 397–409. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, et al. (1979) Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Benore E, D’Auria A, Banez GA, et al. (2015) The influence of anxiety reduction on clinical response to pediatric chronic pain rehabilitation. The Clinical Journal of Pain 31(5): 375–383. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burns JW, Day MA, Thorn BE. (2012) Is reduction in pain catastrophizing a therapeutic mechanism specific to cognitive-behavioral therapy for chronic pain? Translational Behavioral Medicine 2(1): 22–29. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cameron LD. (2003) Anxiety, cognition, and responses to health threats. In: Cameron LD, Leventhal H. (eds) The Self-Regulation of Health and Illness Behaviour, vol. 1 London: Routledge, pp. 157–183. [Google Scholar]
  • Clark DA, Beck AT. (2010) Cognitive theory and therapy of anxiety and depression: Convergence with neurobiological findings. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 14(9): 418–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Crombez G, Vlaeyen JW, Heuts PH, et al. (1999) Pain-related fear is more disabling than pain itself: Evidence on the role of pain-related fear in chronic back pain disability. Pain 80(1–2): 329–339. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • David D. (2003) Rational Emotive Behaviour Therapy (REBT): The view of a cognitive psychologist. In: Dryden W. (ed.) Rational Emotive Behaviour Therapy: Theoretical Developments. New York: Brunner-Routledge, pp. 130–159. [Google Scholar]
  • David D, Cristea I, Hofmann SG. (2018) Why cognitive behavioral therapy is the current gold standard of psychotherapy. Frontiers in Psychiatry 9: 4. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • David D, Hofmann SG. (2013) Another error of descartes? Implications for the “third wave” cognitive-behavioral therapy. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies 13(1): 115–124. [Google Scholar]
  • David D, Freeman A, Di Giuseppe R. (2010) Rational and irrational beliefs research, theory, and clinical practice. In: David D, Lynn SJ, Ellis A. (eds) Rational and Irrational Beliefs: Implications for Mechanisms of Change and Practice in Psychotherapy. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 195–217. [Google Scholar]
  • David D, Montgomery GH, Lorenzo T. (2006) Response expectancy versus response hope in predicting distress. A brief research report. Erdélyi Pszichológiai Szemle, VII, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  • David D, Montgomery GH, Stan R, et al. (2004) Discrimination between hopes and expectancies for nonvolitional outcomes: Psychological phenomenon or artifact? Personality and Individual Differences 36: 1945–1952. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • David D, Szentagotai A, Lupu V, et al. (2008) Rational emotive behavior therapy, cognitive therapy, and medication in the treatment of major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial, posttreatment outcomes, and six-month follow-up. Journal of Clinical Psychology 64(6): 728–746. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dindo L, Van Liew JR, Arch JJ. (2017) Acceptance and commitment therapy: A transdiagnostic behavioral intervention for mental health and medical conditions. Neurotherapeutics 14(3): 546–553. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • DiMatteo MR, Lepper HS, Croghan TW. (2000) Depression is a risk factor for noncompliance with medical treatment: Meta-analysis of the effects of anxiety and depression on patient adherence. Archives of Internal Medicine 160(14): 2101–2107. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dolce JJ, Crocker MF, Moletteire C, et al. (1986) Exercise quotas, anticipatory concern and self-efficacy expectancies in chronic pain: A preliminary report. Pain 24(3): 365–372. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Dryden W. (2005) Rational emotive behavior therapy. In: Nezu CM, Nezu AM, Reinecke MA. (eds) Encyclopedia of Cognitive Behavior Therapy. Boston, MA: Springer, pp. 321–324. [Google Scholar]
  • Dryden W, David D, Ellis A. (2010) Rational Emotive Behavior Therapy: Handbook of Cognitive-Behavioral Therapies (3rd edn). New York: Guilford press. [Google Scholar]
  • Duivenvoorden T, Vissers MM, Verhaar JAN, et al. (2013) Anxiety and depressive symptoms before and after total hip and knee arthroplasty: A prospective multicentre study. Osteoarthritis and Cartilage 21(12): 1834–1840. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Eccleston C, Jordan A, McCracken LM, et al. (2005) The Bath Adolescent Pain Questionnaire (BAPQ): Development and preliminary psychometric evaluation of an instrument to assess the impact of chronic pain on adolescents. Pain 118(1–2): 263–270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Edwards RR, Bingham CO, Bathon J, et al. (2006) Catastrophizing and pain in arthritis, fibromyalgia, and other rheumatic diseases. Arthritis Care & Research 55(2): 325–332. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ellis A. (1962) Reason and Emotion in Psychotherapy. Oxford: Lyle Stuart. [Google Scholar]
  • Garratt G, Ingram RE, Rand KL, et al. (2007) Cognitive processes in cognitive therapy: Evaluation of the mechanisms of change in the treatment of depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice 14(3): 224–239. [Google Scholar]
  • Gatchel RJ, Reuben DB, Dagenais S, et al. (2018) Research agenda for the prevention of pain and its impact: Report of the work group on the prevention of acute and chronic pain of the federal pain research strategy. The Journal of Pain 19(8): 837–851. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harvey AG, Watkins E, Mansell W, et al. (2004) Cognitive Behavioural Processes across Psychological Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Approach to Research and Treatment. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Hsu YW, Somma J, Hung YC, et al. (2005) Predicting postoperative pain by preoperative pressure pain assessment. Anesthesiology: The Journal of the American Society of Anesthesiologists 103(3): 613–618. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ingram RE, Luxton DD. (2005) Vulnerability-stress models. In: Hankin BL, Abela JRZ. (eds) Development of Psychopathology: A Vulnerability-Stress Perspective. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE, pp. 32–46. [Google Scholar]
  • Kirsch I. (1985) Response expectancy as a determinant of experience and behavior. American Psychologist 40(11): 1189–1202. [Google Scholar]
  • Koechlin H, Coakley R, Schechter N, et al. (2018) The role of emotion regulation in chronic pain: A systematic literature review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 107: 38–45. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Krishnamurthy P, Carter P, Blair E. (2001) Attribute framing and goal framing effects in health decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 85(2): 382–399. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kwekkeboom KL, Cherwin CH, Lee JW, et al. (2010) Mind-body treatments for the pain-fatigue-sleep disturbance symptom cluster in persons with cancer. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management 39(1): 126–138. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lauriola M, Tomai M, Palma R, et al. (2019) Intolerance of uncertainty and anxiety-related dispositions predict pain during upper endoscopy. Frontiers in Psychology 10: 1112. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Le Borgne M, Boudoukha AH, Petit A, et al. (2017) Chronic low back pain and the transdiagnostic process: How do cognitive and emotional dysregulations contribute to the intensity of risk factors and pain? Scandinavian Journal of Pain 17: 309–315. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Leahy RL. (2015) Emotional Schema Therapy. New York: Guilford Publications. [Google Scholar]
  • Leeuw M, Goossens ME, Linton SJ, et al. (2007) The fear-avoidance model of musculoskeletal pain: Current state of scientific evidence. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 30(1): 77–94. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lin CS, Niddam DM, Hsu ML, et al. (2013) Pain catastrophizing is associated with dental pain in a stressful context. Journal of Dental Research 92(2): 130–135. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Linton SJ. (2013) A transdiagnostic approach to pain and emotion. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research 18(2): 82–103. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Linton SJ, Götestam KG. (1985) Controlling pain reports through operant conditioning: A laboratory demonstration. Perceptual and Motor Skills 60(2): 427–437. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Linton SJ, Flink IK, Schrooten MG, et al. (2016) Understanding co-occurring emotion and pain: The role of context sensitivity from a transdiagnostic perspective. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy 46(3): 129–137. [Google Scholar]
  • Locher C, Nascimento AF, Kossowsky J, et al. (2019) Open-label placebo response —Does optimism matter? A secondary-analysis of a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 116: 25–30. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McHugh RK, Murray HW, Barlow DH. (2009) Balancing fidelity and adaptation in the dissemination of empirically-supported treatments: The promise of transdiagnostic interventions. Behaviour Research and Therapy 47(11): 946–953. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Masedo AI, Esteve MR. (2007) Effects of suppression, acceptance and spontaneous coping on pain tolerance, pain intensity and distress. Behaviour Research and Therapy 45(2): 199–209. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Meesters A, Vancleef LMG, Peters ML. (2019) Emotional flexibility and recovery from pain. Motivation and Emotion 43: 493–504. [Google Scholar]
  • Mehl S, Werner D, Lincoln TM. (2015) Does Cognitive Behavior Therapy for psychosis (CBTp) show a sustainable effect on delusions? A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1450. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Merskey H, Bogduk N. (1994) Task force on taxonomy of the international association for the study of pain. In: Merskey H, Bogduk N. (eds) Classification of Chronic Pain: Descriptions of Chronic Pain Syndromes and Definitions of Pain Terms. Seattle, WA: IASP Press, pp. 180–196. [Google Scholar]
  • Moldofsky H, Chester WJ. (1970) Pain and mood patterns in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Psychosomatic Medicine 32: 309–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Montgomery GH, David D, DiLorenzo TA, et al. (2007) Response expectancies and irrational beliefs predict exam-related distress. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy 25(1): 17–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nicholls JL, Azam MA, Burns LC, et al. (2018) Psychological treatments for the management of postsurgical pain: A systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Patient Related Outcome Measures 9: 49–64. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pakarinen M, Vanhanen S, Sinikallio S, et al. (2014) Depressive burden is associated with a poorer surgical outcome among lumbar spinal stenosis patients: A 5-year follow-up study. The Spine Journal 14(10): 2392–2396. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Payne JW, Bettman JR, Johnson EJ. (1992) Behavioral decision research: A constructive processing perspective. Annual Review of Psychology 43(1): 87–131. [Google Scholar]
  • Philips HC. (1988) The Psychological Management of Chronic Pain: A Treatment Manual. New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • Polit DF, Beck CT. (2010) Generalization in quantitative and qualitative research: Myths and strategies. International Journal of Nursing Studies 47(11): 1451–1458. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • R Core Team (2012) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available at: http://www.R-project.org/
  • Riddle EJ, Smith JH. (2019) New daily persistent headache: A diagnostic and therapeutic odyssey. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports 19(5): 21. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rief W, Glombiewski JA, Gollwitzer M, et al. (2015) Expectancies as core features of mental disorders. Current Opinion in Psychiatry 28(5): 378–385. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Robinson OJ, Vytal K, Cornwell BR, et al. (2013) The impact of anxiety upon cognition: Perspectives from human threat of shock studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 203. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rosseel Y. (2012) lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 48(2): 1–36. Available at: http://www.jstatsoft.org/v48/i02/ [Google Scholar]
  • Roebuck GS, Urquhart DM, Knox L, et al. (2018) Psychological factors associated with ultramarathon runners’ supranormal pain tolerance: A pilot study. The Journal of Pain 19(12): 1406–1415. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Schreiber JB, Nora A, Stage FK, et al. (2006) Reporting structural equation modeling and confirmatory factor analysis results: A review. The Journal of Educational Research 99(6): 323–338. [Google Scholar]
  • Shewchuk RM, Richards JS, Elliott TR. (1998) Dynamic processes in health outcomes among caregivers of patients with spinal cord injuries. Health Psychology 17(2): 125–129. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sibrava NJ, Borkovec TD. (2006) The cognitive avoidance theory of worry. In: Davey GCL, Wells A. (eds) Worry and Its Psychological Disorders: Theory, Assessment and Treatment. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley, pp. 239–256. [Google Scholar]
  • Spielberger CD. (1989) State-trait Anxiety Inventory: A Comprehensive Bibliography. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, et al. (1983) State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). Redwood City, CA: Mind Garden, p; 77. [Google Scholar]
  • Stapinski LA, Abbott MJ, Rapee RM. (2010) Evaluating the cognitive avoidance model of generalised anxiety disorder: Impact of worry on threat appraisal, perceived control and anxious arousal. Behaviour Research and Therapy 48(10): 1032–1040. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J. (1995) The pain catastrophizing scale: Development and validation. Psychological Assessment 7(4): 524–532. [Google Scholar]
  • Sullivan MJ, Rodgers WM, Kirsch I. (2001) Catastrophizing, depression and expectancies for pain and emotional distress. Pain 91(1–2): 147–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sweet SD, McGrath PJ. (1998) Physiological measures of pain. In: Finley GA, McGrath PJ. (eds) Progress in Pain Research and Management, vol. 10 Seattle, WA: IASP Press, pp. 59–82. [Google Scholar]
  • Szentagotai A, Jones J. (2010) The behavioral consequences of irrational beliefs. In: David D, Lynn SJ, Ellis A. (eds) Rational and Irrational Beliefs. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 75–97. [Google Scholar]
  • Tang J, Gibson SJ. (2005) A psychophysical evaluation of the relationship between trait anxiety, pain perception, and induced state anxiety. The Journal of Pain 6(9): 612–619. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tran ST, Jastrowski Mano KE, Hainsworth KR, et al. (2015) Distinct influences of anxiety and pain catastrophizing on functional outcomes in children and adolescents with chronic pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology 40(8): 744–755. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tunks ER, Crook J, Weir R. (2008) Epidemiology of chronic pain with psychological comorbidity: Prevalence, risk, course, and prognosis. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry 53(4): 224–234. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Turk DC, Okifuji A. (2002) Psychological factors in chronic pain: Evolution and revolution. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 70(3): 678–690. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vervoort T, Eccleston C, Goubert L, et al. (2010) Children’s catastrophic thinking about their pain predicts pain and disability 6 months later. European Journal of Pain 14(1): 90–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Vlaeyen JW, Linton SJ. (2000) Fear-avoidance and its consequences in chronic musculoskeletal pain: A state of the art. Pain 85(3): 317–332. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wade JB, Price DD, Hamer RM, et al. (1990) An emotional component analysis of chronic pain. Pain 40(3): 303–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wang Y, Jackson T, Cai L. (2016) Causal effects of threat and challenge appraisals on coping and pain perception. European Journal of Pain 20(7): 1111–1120. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Weston R, Gore PA., Jr (2006) A brief guide to structural equation modeling. The Counseling Psychologist 34(5): 719–751. [Google Scholar]
  • Wewers ME, Lowe NK. (1990) A critical review of visual analogue scales in the measurement of clinical phenomena. Research in Nursing & Health 13(4): 227–236. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wojtyna E. (2012) Irrational suffering—An impact of cognitive behavioural therapy on the depression level and the perception of pain in cancer patients

    What is behavior is motivated by forces that individuals may not understand or be aware of?

    The mind can be viewed as an information processing system. Behavior is motivated by forces that individuals may not understand or be aware of. Human thought and behavior can be understood in terms of activity at the neural level.

    Which perspective would most likely suggest that behaviour may be the product of unconscious conflict and anxiety?

    Originating in the work of Sigmund Freud, the psychodynamic perspective emphasizes unconscious psychological processes (for example, wishes and fears of which we're not fully aware), and contends that childhood experiences are crucial in shaping adult personality.

    What perspective is psychological science is the study of behavior that is observable and measurable?

    The behavioral perspective belongs to a school of thought known as behaviorism or behavioral theory. Behavioral theory is the overarching analysis of human behavior focused on examining a person's environment and learned associations.

    Which perspective suggests that underlying conflicts influence behavior?

    The psychodynamic perspective in psychology proposes that there are psychological forces underlying human behaviour, feelings, and emotions.