According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Introduction

The accurate measurement of time is a biological necessity for all organisms, allowing them to align their internal biological cycles with the external cycles on which they depend for survival. Animals use a variety of biological mechanisms to measure time on scales ranging from microseconds to years (Buonomano, 2007). Out of the many ways that biology has found to measure time, the one that is of most direct relevance to behaviour and cognition is that which spans the duration from milliseconds to minutes (Matell & Meck, 2000). This area of timing, often referred to as interval timing, is characterised by both a relatively low level of accuracy and a high level of flexibility in measuring intervals on demand (Gibbon et al., 1997).

Given the importance of interval timing, it is surprising to find that its neurobiological mechanisms are still poorly understood (Matell & Meck, 2000). This uncertainty has led to a debate between several rival models describing different mechanisms for timing, each of which predicts a different mathematical relationship between durations of physical time and measures of perceived time (Grondin, 2001).

In time-perception research, estimations of duration are treated as measurements of the perceived intensity of the stimulus of time in a way that is analogous to the intensity of any other physical stimulus (Grondin, 2001). As with all types of perception, organisms are unable to perceive variations in duration if those changes fall below a certain threshold, known as the just noticeable difference (JND). Analysis of the way that these perceptual thresholds change as duration changes can yield useful information about the nature of the processes that an organism uses to measure time (Grondin, 2001). This analysis is informed by models that attempt to predict the relationship between stimulus threshold and stimulus intensity (Grondin, 2010a).

The relationship between threshold and intensity is described by Weber’s law (Sowden, 2012). In its strict form, Weber’s law predicts that the ratio of JND to stimulus intensity (I) will be constant:

$$ \frac{JND}{I}=k, $$

(1)

where the term JND/I is known as the Weber fraction (Wf) and k is known as the Weber constant (Holway & Pratt, 1936). Weber’s law holds for a wide variety of stimuli across a broad range of intensities (Sowden, 2012); however, it is also violated in many instances (Masin, 2009). For example, many types of stimuli exhibit disproportionately low sensitivity to changes in stimulus intensity at very low stimulus intensity (Sowden, 2012). Although Weber’s law is not universal, the Wf is still widely used as a measure of stimulus sensitivity in models that seek to describe the mechanisms behind experimentally observed variations in perceptual thresholds (Gibbon et al., 1997).

Stimulus thresholds can be derived by asking subjects to discriminate between two stimuli and then plotting the percentage of correct discriminations against stimulus intensity to generate a psychometric function (Kingdom & Prins, 2016). By defining the JND in terms of the slope of the psychometric function, the variability of the perceptual discriminations becomes a measure of sensitivity to changes in stimulus intensity (Grondin, 2010b). Definition of the JND thus allows the Wf to be stated as a coefficient of variation in terms of the ratio between the standard deviation (SD) of perceptual discriminations and the mean (M) of those discriminations:

$$ Wf=\frac{SD}{M} $$

(2)

The way that sensitivity to changes in stimulus intensity varies across a specific range can be visualised by plotting the Wf against stimulus intensity. The resulting function, the perceptual sensitivity function (PSF), can exhibit a variety of shapes depending on the way that the Wf varies with changes in stimulus intensity (Lejeune & Wearden, 2006). Weber’s law predicts that Wfs will be constant across all intensity values, and therefore in situations where Weber’s law is supported, the PSF will be flat. In contrast, where stimulus sensitivity is very low at low stimulus intensity but constant at higher intensities, the PSF will have a reverse J shape, with Wfs starting high and falling to a horizontal asymptote.

The applicability of Weber’s law to the relationship between duration and the perception of duration has been the subject of ongoing debate in the literature (e.g., Bizo et al., 2006; Getty, 1975; Grondin, 2014; Haß et al., 2008; Killeen & Weiss, 1987). Scalar expectancy theory predicts that measurements of stimulus thresholds for time perception will exhibit constant Wfs and therefore flat PSFs (Gibbon & Church, 1984), a relationship that has come to be known as the scalar property of time perception (Grondin, 2014). This relationship can be stated mathematically by replacing M with the mean duration of the temporal discriminations (t̄) in Eq. 2 (Gibbon, 1977).

$$ Wf=\frac{SD}{\overline{t}}=k $$

(3)

There is good evidence, however, that time perception is not entirely scalar, but violates Weber’s law at very short intervals, with Wfs that fall from a high value to a horizontal asymptote at a point somewhere between 50 and 2,000 ms (Church et al., 1976; Corke et al., 2018; Fetterman & Killeen, 1992; Getty, 1975). Getty (1975) proposed a generalised form of Weber’s law that models this characteristic reverse J-shaped PSF according to the equation:

$$ Wf=\frac{\sqrt{A{\overline{t}}^2+C}}{\overline{t}}, $$

(4)

where the parameter C represents a component of residual noise variance, A is a parameter related to the value of the Weber constant, and Wf and t̄ are as defined above (for a derivation of this equation, see the Online Supplementary Materials (OSM)).

Another prominent model developed to describe the relationship between stimulus sensitivity and stimulus intensity in time perception is that of Killeen and Weiss (1987). This model represents the variability in subjects’ ability to measure time in terms of the advantage that they gain from segmenting intervals into subintervals in a way that minimises variance. The result is a quadratic relation in which the PSF is given by:

$$ Wf=\frac{\sqrt{A{\overline{t}}^2+B\overline{t}+C}}{\overline{t}}, $$

(5)

where A, B, and C are free parameters (Killeen & Weiss, 1987; see Online Supplementary Materials for derivation). The strength of this model is that it accommodates many previously developed models as special cases. For example, Eq. 5 becomes Weber’s law (Eq. 3) with B = C = 0, and it becomes Getty’s model (Eq. 4) with B = 0 (Killeen & Weiss, 1987).

The model proposed by Killeen and Weiss (1987) can be used to describe both the flat PSFs and the reverse J-shaped PSFs found in the experimental literature. This model assumes that the scalar property of time perception is only violated at shorter intervals and holds at longer intervals. The majority of studies in both the human- and animal-timing literature support this assumption (Lejeune & Wearden, 2006; Wearden & Lejeune, 2008). There is, however, some evidence to suggest that it may not be universally correct, with several studies showing rising Wfs at longer intervals (Grondin, 2010b, 2012; Lavoie & Grondin, 2004; Lejeune & Wearden, 1991). Conjoined with earlier findings of falling Wfs at shorter intervals, this evidence suggests that the overall shape of the PSF may, at least in some circumstances, be U-shaped rather than reverse J-shaped, falling at shorter intervals only to rise again at longer intervals.

Perhaps the most well-known example of U-shaped PSFs comes from Getty (1975). A two-alternative forced-choice interval comparison paradigm was used to measure temporal perception thresholds in two human subjects at a range of durations from 50 to 3,200 ms. Wfs were highest at 50 ms and levelled out at around 200 ms; however, careful examination of Getty’s data reveals that Wfs began to rise again somewhere around 2,500 ms (Fig. 1). A similar result in a different range of intervals had been reported many years earlier by Woodrow (1930), who measured perceptual thresholds from 0.2 to 30 s using a temporal reproduction task in eight male subjects. Woodrow found Wfs that decreased slightly from 0.2 to 0.6 s, remained constant to 1.5 s, and then increased beyond 1.5 s.

Fig. 1

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Perceptual sensitivity functions (PSF) plotting Weber fractions (SD/T) against interval duration (T) for the two subjects in Getty’s (1975) study fitted to a counter model (dashed line) and Getty’s model (Eq. 4; solid line). Datapoints are for SIs of (from left to right) 50, 100, 200, 400, 600, 800, 1,000, 1,200, 1,400, 1,600, 1,800, 2,000, 2,400, 2,800, and 3,200 ms. Reprinted from “Discrimination of short temporal intervals: A comparison of two models,” by D. J. Getty, 1975, Perception & Psychophysics, 18, p.5. Copyright 1975 by The Psychonomic Society. Reprinted with permission

Full size image

U-shaped PSFs are not limited to the human timing literature. Cantor and Wilson (1981) found a U-shaped PSF with low points from 0.5 to 2 s in rats performing a temporal reproduction task across a range of intervals from 0.2 to 6 s. More recently, U-shaped PSFs were found in pigeons using both temporal production and interval comparison paradigms across durations from 0.5 to 64 s (Bizo et al., 2006), and in domestic dogs using a temporal bisection paradigm across intervals from 0.5 to 16 s (Cliff et al., 2019).

This small body of experimental evidence for the existence of U-shaped PSFs is problematic. Even the most generalised model of time perception (Killeen & Weiss, 1987) does not accommodate data with Wfs that increase at longer intervals. In an attempt to fill this gap, Bizo et al. (2006) modified the Killeen and Weiss (1987) model to describe the U-shaped PSF generated in their study, yielding a Wf given by:

$$ Wf=\frac{\sqrt{A{\overline{t}}^m+B\overline{t}+C}}{\overline{t}}, $$

(6)

where m is an additional free parameter that allows the exponent in Eq. 5 to vary its value to fit the data.

Information about the shape of the PSF is important in timing research because it informs the development of models that seek to describe the neurological and cognitive processes that give rise to the perception of time (Grondin, 2010a). Many of these models rely on the assumption that Wfs remain constant at longer intervals (Matell & Meck, 2000). However, the research cited above has established that time perception is not always scalar at longer intervals (Grondin, 2010b, 2012; Lavoie & Grondin, 2004; Lejeune & Wearden, 1991). These deviations from the scalar property are made manifest by the U-shaped PSFs that are generated by both humans and animals under some conditions (Bizo et al., 2006; Cantor & Wilson, 1981; Cliff et al., 2019; Getty, 1975; Woodrow, 1930).

Getty’s (1975) data, showing that human PSFs rise after about 2,500 ms (Fig. 1), has been cited as evidence of the violation of the scalar property of time perception at longer intervals (e.g., Bizo et al., 2006; Grondin, 2001, 2010b; Haß et al., 2008; Lavoie & Grondin, 2004). The results of human studies by Woodrow (1930), Grondin (2010b, 2012), and Lavoie and Grondin (2004) suggest that this rise might begin as early as 1,200 ms. Other studies have failed to find rising PSFs in human subjects (Wearden & Lejeune, 2008). Is this rise a reliable effect? And if so, at what point in the overall range of millisecond to minutes scale timing does it occur?

We aimed to explore the anomaly in Getty’s (1975) data to determine whether variations in temporal sensitivity in humans are best described by a reverse J-shaped or U-shaped PSF. We used a methodology similar to that utilised by Getty to generate Wfs across a range of intervals from 100 to 3,200 ms. The resulting data were then fit to the generalised model of Killeen and Weiss (1987; Eq. 5) and the variant of that model developed by Bizo et al. (2006; Eq. 6). We hypothesised that the PSF generated from this dataset would be U-shaped. We also hypothesised that the best fit for this function would be given by the model developed by Bizo et al., which is the only extant model capable of describing U-shaped PSFs.

Method

Participants

The sample consisted of 24 participants, 14 of whom were female (58%). Participants ranged in age from 24 to 73 years (M = 38.13, SD = 10.18), had adequate hearing for the experimental task, and were able to understand written instructions in English.

Participants were recruited via an invitation circulated through the online social media platform Facebook and gave consent via an electronic form presented at the beginning of the experiment. The Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of New England approved this study (HE19-075).

Apparatus and materials

The experiment was carried out using a custom-made script running on version 5.0 of the Inquisit software platform (Millisecond, 2018). The scrip is available in the OSM. The stimuli were defined by the start and stop points of a series of pure 440-Hz tones of different durations. These tones were generated using version 8.5 of the professional digital audio workstation Cubase (Steinberg, 2015) and recorded as WAV files (available in the Online Supplementary Material).

The stimuli consisted of two types of intervals, standard intervals (SIs) and test intervals (TIs), which were identical in all aspects apart from their duration. The six experimental conditions (S1–S6) were defined by the six SIs, which were distributed in logarithmic increments from 100 to 3,200 ms. For each SI there were five TIs, consisting of a central TI equal to the duration of the SI itself and two TIs either side of the SI spaced at durations proportional to the magnitude of the SI (Table 1).

Table 1 Duration in milliseconds of the five test intervals (TIs) for each of the six standard intervals (SIs) used in the experiment

Full size table

The experiment was run on a Lenovo Yoga 520 laptop, and the audio stimuli were delivered binaurally using standard Audio-Technica ATH-M20x headphones at an A-weighted sound level of 55 dB. Responses were indicated using the left and right arrow keys on the computer keyboard.

Procedure

Participants were tested individually in single sessions lasting between 60 and 90 min. The experiment was conducted in a small enclosed office with participants seated facing away from the windows to minimise the risk of distraction. The A-weighted ambient sound level in the room was 32.6 dB, and the light intensity was 411 lx.

The experiment utilised a two-alternative forced-choice interval comparison task similar to that used by Getty (1975). In each trial, a single pair of stimuli consisting of an SI and a TI were presented. Participants were required to decide which of the two intervals was longer. The experiment consisted of six blocks of 100 trials each (Fig. 2, Panel A). Each experimental block tested one of the six SIs — each corresponding to one of the six experimental conditions. The order of the blocks was permuted using a balanced Latin square to minimise the risk of order effects between the six conditions. Each block consisted of 20 randomly distributed comparisons between each of the five TIs and the SI of that condition (Fig. 2, Panel B). The order of presentation of the SI and TI was randomly varied to avoid interval order effects (Jamieson & Petrusic, 1975). Interstimulus intervals were varied randomly from 750 to 850 ms, and the post-stimulus interval was varied randomly from 350 to 450 ms so that there were no regular intervals in the experiment apart from the experimental stimuli.

Fig. 2

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Schematic depiction of the structure of the overall experiment (Panel A) and a single trial (Panel B)

Full size image

Each experimental session was initiated with two short training blocks of 20 trials each. In the first training block, which was designed to test participants’ understanding of the instructions, the intervals were easily distinguishable, and feedback was given after each response. In the second training block, the intervals were identical to those of the third (S3) condition, and there was no feedback following each response by a participant. During these two training blocks, the experimenter watched from outside the room to be available to answer any questions. Participants were then left to complete the remainder of the experiment. All participants performed satisfactorily on the first block of practice trials and were able to complete the experiment without assistance.

Participants were instructed at the beginning of each block not to tap, count, or use any other periodic movements to measure the intervals. This was done to reduce the likelihood that participants would use counting as a mediating strategy. The level of participants’ confidence in their ability to comply with this instruction was assessed at the end of each block using a five-point Likert scale between “very uncertain” and “very certain”.

At the end of each block, participants were able to take a break for whatever duration they desired, and refreshments were available in the experiment room throughout the experiment. Participants were debriefed and allowed to ask additional questions about the experiment at the end of the experiment.

Data analysis

Initial data analysis was conducted using Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Office 365 Pro Plus, Version 16.0) and its Solver addons. Three metrics for response accuracy, practice effects, and response bias were calculated to assess the validity of the data. Response accuracy was assessed by recording a value of 1 for correct responses and 0 for incorrect responses in each trial. Because the third TI was identical to the SI and therefore neither response could be correct or incorrect, responses to this TI were assigned a value of 0.5 in this accuracy metric regardless of the judgement made. This ensured that random responding would result in 50% accuracy in this metric. (Note that these data were not used for calculating the Wf.) Practice effects were assessed by creating a separate practice metric and assigning a value of 1 to each correct judgement, excluding the third TI. These values were totalled over the entire experiment to generate a cumulative number correct, which was plotted against the trial number and assessed for linearity using least-squares linear regression. Response biases were assessed using the response values recorded in the raw data to generate a value for the percentage of right-arrow responses in each block.

The raw data used to generate the Wfs consisted of 20 binary discriminations (SI longer or TI longer) for each of the 30 TIs. Discriminations were given a value of 0 if the participant judged the SI to be the longer of each pair of intervals and a value of 1 if the participant judged the TI to be longer. These values were summed across the 20 trials of each of the five TIs in each block. They were then converted into a percentage, yielding a value for the percentage of long responses for each TI. These percentage long values were plotted against TI duration within each block to give six psychometric functions for each participant. The standard deviation for each of these psychometric functions was calculated directly from a frequency distribution consisting of the time intervals for which “longer” judgements were made in each block, and the Wf for each condition was calculated by dividing the standard deviation by the mean discrimination duration (t̄).

The independent variable for our primary analysis was the duration of the SIs in the six conditions, while the dependent variable was the Wf generated in each condition. To assess the degree of difference between these six Wfs, a repeated-measures analysis of variance was conducted using the SPSS software package (IBM SPSS Statistics, Version 23). PSFs for each participant were generated by plotting the Wf against t̄ for each condition. An overall mean PSF was generated by plotting the mean of all participant’s Wfs against overall mean t̄ for each condition (see Fig. 4).

A coefficient of determination (r2) describing the degree of fit between these PSFs and the Killeen and Weiss (1987; Eq. 5) and Bizo et al. (2006; Eq. 6) models was calculated according to the non-linear regression procedure outlined by Brown (2001) using the Solver plug-in in Microsoft Excel (Microsoft Office 365 Pro Plus, Version 16.0). The difference in the number of free parameters in the two models made interpretation of coefficients of determination problematic (Eqs. 5 and 6 with three and four free parameters, respectively; Spiess & Neumeyer, 2010). Consequently, the final analysis was conducted using corrected Akaike information criterion (AICc) values (Burnham & Anderson, 2004; for mathematical details, see the OSM).

Results

Mean discrimination durations (t̄) were consistently close to the SI duration (OSM, Fig. S1), and all mean psychometric functions had a positive slope (Fig. 3). The six psychometric functions for each of the 24 participants (see OSM, Fig. S2) also had a positive slope, apart from two cases with zero slopes and one with a slightly negative slope (participant 1, S1 condition; Fig. S2). Replacing the latter Wf with a value corresponding to a zero slope did not affect the outcome, so all psychometric functions were included in the data analysis.

Fig. 3

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Mean psychometric functions (black dots) plotting the mean percentage of trials in which the test interval (TI) was judged longer than the standard interval against TI duration for conditions S1–S6, with 95% confidence intervals (CIs; dashed lines)

Full size image

Wfs ranged from 0.05 to 0.12 (OSM, Table S2), and mean Wfs were between 0.09 and 0.10 (M = 0.09, SD = 0.01; Table 2). The overall mean PSF was slightly concave (Fig. 4, black dots). A one-way repeated-measures analysis of variance showed that there was a significant difference in the Wfs between the six conditions, F(5, 115) = 4.69, p = .001, with a large effect size (partial η2 = .17). Pairwise comparisons using the Bonferroni correction revealed that the Wfs for the shortest condition (S1) were significantly higher than those of the third, fourth, and fifth conditions (p < .05; Table 3). Although the mean Wf for the longest (S6) condition was also higher than that of the third, fourth, and fifth conditions (Table 2), the difference was not statistically significant, and no other comparisons were statistically significant (Table 3).

Table 2 Mean Weber fractions for each condition with standard deviations

Full size table

Fig. 4

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Mean perceptual sensitivity function plotting the mean Weber fractions (Wf) against the overall mean of the mean discrimination durations for each condition (black dots) fit to the functions of the Bizo et al. (2006) model (Eq. 6; Panel A), the Killeen and Weiss (1987) model (Eq. 5; Panel B), the Getty (1975) model (Eq. 4; Panel C), and the Weber’s law model (Eq. 3; Panel D) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs; dashed lines)

Full size image

Table 3 Results for the one-way analysis of variance comparing the Weber fractions between the six conditions

Full size table

The Bizo et al. (2006) model (Eq. 6) had a higher coefficient of determination when fit to the overall PSF (r2 = 0.80) than the Killeen and Weiss (1987) model (Eq. 5; r2 = 0.60). The Bizo et al. model also had a higher mean coefficient of determination when fit to the individual PSFs (mean r2 = 0.46, SD = 0.28) than the Killeen and Weiss model (mean r2 = 0.26, SD = 0.30; Table 4; for individual PSFs with 95% confidence intervals see OSM, Fig. S2). The Bizo et al. model gave a better fit to the data than the Killeen and Weiss model when no adjustments are made for the difference in the number of free parameters between the two models. There was a high degree of variation in the coefficient of determination values, ranging from 0 to 0.95 for the Bizo et al. model and -0.34 to 0.95 for the Killeen and Weiss model (Fig. 5).

Table 4 Results for the comparison between the Bizo et al. (2006; Eq. 6) model and the Killeen and Weiss (1987; Eq. 5), Getty (1975; Eq. 4), and Weber’s law (Eq. 3) models for the overall model fit and the mean of individual model fits. Note that r2 is not a good indicator of model fit for Weber’s law because the model is directly proportional to the mean, however, this does not affect the Akaike information criterion (AICc) value

Full size table

Fig. 5

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Coefficients of determination for each participant for the Killeen and Weiss (1987; black bars) and Bizo et al. (2006; grey bars) models

Full size image

AICc values for the fit to the overall PSF were higher for the Bizo et al. model than for the Killeen and Weiss model (Table 4, upper portion), with an AICc difference value between the two models of 28.21. The mean AICc values for the fit to the individual PSFs were also higher for the Bizo et al. model than for the Killeen and Weiss model (Table 4, lower portion), with an AICc difference value between the two models of 29.09. This demonstrates that the Killeen and Weiss model gave the best fit to the data when adjusted for the difference in the number of free parameters between the two models (Burnham & Anderson, 2004).

The mean percentage correct for each condition was consistently above 60% (M = 69%, SD = 5.46; Fig. 6). A one-sample t-test comparing the percentage correct with the 50% value expected with random responding found that mean percent correct differed significantly from chance in all six conditions (p < .001). Plots of the cumulative number of trials with correct responses across the experiment for each participant (see OSM, Fig. S3) showed an almost perfectly linear accuracy pattern for all participants (mean r2 = 1.00), indicating that there were no learning effects in this experiment.

Fig. 6

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Overall mean percentage of correct responses for each condition. Individual scores are represented by black circles, slightly jittered for clarity; shaded areas represent 95% Highest Density Intervals (HDIs), calculated using R's BEST (Bayesian Estimation Supersedes the T-Test) package, and vertical bars represent the 10th and 90th quantiles

Full size image

The results of the assessment of response bias were mixed. The overall percentage of right-arrow responses was 50% (SD = 6.04), which is the value expected from a bias-free response pattern; however, right-arrow responses were at their lowest number in the shortest interval condition, M = 39.83, SD = 12.69, and climbed steadily to their highest number in the longest interval condition, M = 63.04, SD = 10.02 (Fig. 7). A one-sample t-test comparing the percentage of right-arrow responses with a bias-free performance of 50% showed that the bias was significant in both the S1 condition, t(23) = - 3.93, p = .001, d = 0.80 and the S6 condition, t(23) = 6.37, p < .001, d = 1.30.

Fig. 7

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Mean percentage of right-arrow responses for each condition. Individual scores are represented by black circles, slightly jittered for clarity; shaded areas represent 95% Highest Density Intervals (HDIs), calculated using R's BEST (Bayesian Estimation Supersedes the T-Test) package, and vertical bars represent the 10th and 90th quantiles

Full size image

Participants tended to report a high level of confidence that they were not counting or using any other rhythmic strategies in this experiment (M = 4.0, SD = 1.18). Mean confidence levels were highest in the S1 condition (M = 4.54, SD = 0.98), and declined to their lowest level in the S6 condition (M = 3.58, SD = 1.44; Fig. 8). A Pearson’s correlation between participants’ coefficients of determination in the fit to the Bizo et al. (2006) model and their responses to the confidence question for the longest (S6) interval condition revealed a weak correlation that was not statistically significant, r(24) = .18, p = .396.

Fig. 8

According to webers law, a person would be more likely to detect the difference between:

Mean response to the question “How certain are you that you did not count, tap, or use any other periodic movements to measure the duration of the intervals in this block of trials?” (Appendix D, Fig. D17) as measured on a five-point Likert scale where 1 = “very uncertain” and 5 = “very certain”. Individual scores are represented by black circles, slightly jittered for clarity; shaded areas represent 95% Highest Density Intervals (HDIs), calculated using R's BEST (Bayesian Estimation Supersedes the T-Test) package, and vertical bars represent the 10th and 90th quantiles

Full size image

Discussion

Overall, participants in this study were more likely to judge the TI as being longer than the SI as TI duration increased (OSM, Fig. S1), which demonstrates that the stimuli were within the intended range of participants’ sensitivity to duration. Participants also exhibited high accuracy in their temporal discriminations, with consistently low Wfs (see OSM, Table S2) falling within a range similar to that found in Getty’s (1975) original study (Fig. 1).

The hypothesis that the PSF would be U-shaped was only weakly supported. Visually, the mean PSF had a slightly concave shape (Fig. 4), and mean Wfs were marginally higher in the two shortest (S1 and S2) and the longest (S6) conditions (Table 2); however, the slight upturn in the longest interval, which is the crucial element in demonstrating a U-shaped PSF, was not statistically significant (Table 3). Furthermore, the PSFs of individual participants did not show any consistent pattern (see OSM, Fig. S2), whereas the PSFs of both subjects in Getty’s (1975) study are U-shaped (Fig. 1). Visual inspection of Fig. 1 demonstrates that the drop in Wfs at short intervals found in this study is smaller than that found by Getty; however, part of this early drop in Wfs reported by Getty occurs between the 50- and 100-ms intervals, whereas the 50-ms interval was not included in this study. Ignoring the first data point in both panels of Fig. 1, the main difference between these two functions and the mean PSF found in this study (Fig. 4) is the markedly lower magnitude of the rise in Wfs at longer intervals in the latter compared with the former.

The hypothesis that the Bizo et al. (2006) model (Eq. 6) would give the best fit to the data was also only weakly supported. When using the coefficient of determination as the metric of comparison, the Bizo et al. model gave a better fit to both the mean PSF and the individual PSFs. This result, however, did not hold for the comparison of the AICc values, which adjust for the different number of free parameters in the two models (Burnham & Anderson, 2004). The magnitude of the AICc differences between these two models for both the mean PSF and the individual PSFs are high enough to conclude that the hypothesis that the Bizo et al. model would give a better fit to the data has no empirical support (Burnham et al., 2011). Therefore, although allowing the exponent of the first term in the Killeen and Weiss (1987) model (Eq. 5) to vary (parameter m in Eq. 6) did yield a higher coefficient of determination for the Bizo et al. model, the increase in the accuracy of the model fit to this dataset was not sufficient to justify the addition of an extra free parameter into the model.

Given that both Getty’s (1975) model (Eq. 4) and Weber’s law (Eq. 3) have fewer free parameters than either the Bizo et al. (2006; Eq. 6) or the Killeen and Weiss (1987; Eq. 5) models, it is useful to explore how the former two models compare to the latter in the model fit to this dataset. A comparison between all four models (Table 4) shows that, although the Bizo et al. model certainly had the highest coefficients of determination of the four, it is Weber’s law and Getty’s (1975) model, the models with the least number of free parameters, that had the lowest AICc values. However, the difference in AICc values between the two models with the lowest AICc values (Getty’s model and Weber’s law) is not large enough to confidently distinguish between these two models (Burnham & Anderson, 1998). Therefore, the final outcome of this analysis is that Getty’s model (Eq. 4) and Weber’s law (Eq. 3) both provide the best fit to this dataset when the difference in the number of free parameters is taken into account.

There are, however, a few considerations that must qualify any generalisations based on these results. This study sought to examine the general trend in timing accuracy across a specific range of intervals. It did not attempt to address the question of how counting affects timing accuracy across that range. There is conflicting evidence on the effect of counting on the accuracy of temporal judgements in humans (Hinton & Rao, 2004), but it is generally assumed that counting improves accuracy at longer intervals, therefore lowering Wfs (Fetterman & Killeen, 1990). Although participants were encouraged not to count in both the current study and Getty’s original (1975) study, the possibility remains that the difference in the profile of the PSFs between the two studies is the result of a higher level of motivation to comply with this directive in Getty’s participants. This possibility is supported by the results of the confidence question, which show that participants in this study were the least confident that they refrained from counting in the longer intervals (Fig. 8). There was no evidence for a systematic relationship between participant’s perceptions of their ability to resist counting and the fit with the Bizo et al. model, however. To further explore this relationship, subsequent research could look specifically at the effect of counting on timing performance.

The results of this study do not seem to have been affected by random responding or learning effects. There was, however, a significant systematic bias in the responses. Left-arrow responses predominated in the S1 condition while right-arrow responses predominated in the S6 condition. Due to the randomisation in the order of presentation of SIs and TIs, this bias is not indicative of an interval order effect. This result might reflect an inherent bias to associate shorter intervals with the left arrow and longer intervals with the right arrow; however, further research would be required to elucidate the nature of this effect.

This study was conducted with a relatively large sample size that had a good spread of ages and a reasonable gender balance. It is possible, however, that the large age range of the sample (24–73 years) could have created a bias towards the model with the smallest number of parameters by increasing the variability in the temporal estimates and decreasing the separation between the models. This criticism would only apply to the AICc difference between Getty’s (1975) model and Weber’s law, as the differences in AICc values between the Bizo et al. (2006) model, the Killeen and Weiss (1987) model, and Getty’s model (Table 4) are well over the value of 6 required to achieve a relative likelihood value of less than 0.05 (Navakatikyan, 2007). Therefore, the large age range does not threaten the validity of the overall result of this study, but it does further decrease the confidence in the statistical validity of the small AICc difference between Getty’s model and Weber’s law.

The results of this study suggest that the decrease in accuracy of temporal discriminations at longer intervals found by Getty (1975) is not a generalisable effect. In addition, our results and Getty’s results together suggest that different individuals may have different profiles of sensitivity to changes in duration. Further research using a similar procedure on a larger sample of participants could establish whether there are indeed significant individual differences in the profile of the PSF for time, and, if so, what the behavioural and cognitive correlates of these differences might be. Individual differences in sensitivity to time are known to exist. For example, deficits in time perception have been found in a range of psychological and neurological conditions (Gibbon et al., 1997), including Parkinson’s disease (Malapani et al., 1998), Alzheimer’s disease (El Haj & Kapogiannis, 2016), and schizophrenia (Ueda et al., 2018). Because the paradigm used in this experiment was designed to run on a standard commercially available software platform, the current study provides a reproducible procedure that could be used to explore variations in the profile of temporal sensitivity across the human population.

Numerous variants of Weber’s law have been proposed to model the profile of sensitivity to changes in the physical stimulus of duration, two of which were compared in this study. Although the most sophisticated of these models (Bizo et al., 2006) provided the best raw fit to the data, the increase in the fit between the two models was not sufficient to warrant the extra free parameter required. Furthermore, accommodations for the difference in the number of free parameters revealed that the two models with the smallest number of free parameters, Getty’s (1975) model and Weber’s law (Holway & Pratt, 1936), actually gave the best fit to the data. This result demonstrates that the decrease in sensitivity to the stimulus of duration found in some previous research at intervals between 1 and 3 s is not a consistent effect. It also adds to the large body of evidence demonstrating that, in certain situations and within a certain range of intervals, the profile of sensitivity to changes in duration is best described by Weber’s law. Thus, at least in the case of this research, Weber’s law appears to have stood the test of time.

References

  • Bizo, L. A., Chu, J. Y. M., Sanabria, F., & Killeen, P. R. (2006). The failure of Weber’s law in time perception and production. Behavioural Processes, 71(2–3), 201–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.11.006

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, A. M. (2001). A step-by-step guide to non-linear regression analysis of experimental data using a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet. Computer Methods and Programs in Biomedicine, 65(3), 191–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-2607(00)00124-3

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Buonomano, D. V. (2007). The biology of time across different scales. Nature Chemical Biology, 3(10), 594–597. https://doi.org/10.1038/nchembio1007-594

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (1998). Model Selection and Multimodel Inference: A Practical Information-Theoretic Approach (2nd). Springer-Verlag. https://www.springer.com/gp/book/9780387953649

  • Burnham, K. P., & Anderson, D. R. (2004). Multimodel Inference: Understanding AIC and BIC in Model Selection. Sociological Methods & Research, 33(2), 261–304. https://doi.org/10.1177/0049124104268644

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnham, K. P., Anderson, D. R., & Huyvaert, K. P. (2011). AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: Some background, observations, and comparisons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65(1), 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1029-6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, M. B., & Wilson, J. F. (1981). Temporal uncertainty as an associative metric: Operant simulations of Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110(2), 232–268. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.232

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. M., Getty, D. J., & Lerner, N. D. (1976). Duration discrimination by rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2(4), 303–312. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.2.4.303

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cliff, J., Jackson, S., McEwan, J., & Bizo, L. (2019). Weber’s Law and the Scalar Property of Timing: A Test of Canine Timing. Animals, 9(10), 801. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9100801

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corke, M., Bell, J., Goodhew, S. C., Smithson, M., & Edwards, M. (2018). Perceived time slows during fleeting fun or fear. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(2), 562–567. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1264000

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • El Haj, M., & Kapogiannis, D. (2016). Time distortions in Alzheimer’s disease: A systematic review and theoretical integration. Npj Aging and Mechanisms of Disease, 2(1), 16016. https://doi.org/10.1038/npjamd.2016.16

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Fetterman, J. G., & Killeen, P. R. (1990). A componential analysis of pacemaker-counter timing systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4), 766–780. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.16.4.766

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fetterman, J. G., & Killeen, P. R. (1992). Time discrimination in Columba livia and Homo sapiens. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 18(1), 80–94. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.18.1.80

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Getty, D. J. (1975). Discrimination of short temporal intervals: A comparison of two models. Perception & Psychophysics, 18(1), 1–8. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199358

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84(3), 279–325.

  • Gibbon, J., & Church, R. M. (1984). Sources of variance in an information processing theory of timing. In H. Roitblat, H. Terrace, & W. Sever (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 465–488). Erlbaum.

  • Gibbon, J., Malapani, C., Dale, C. L., & Gallistel, C. R. (1997). Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: Advances and challenges. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7(2), 170–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4388(97)80005-0

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S. (2001). From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychological Bulletin, 127(1), 22–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.1.22

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S. (2010a). Timing and time perception: A review of recent behavioral and neuroscience findings and theoretical directions. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 561–582. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.3.561

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S. (2010b). Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(5), 1422–1430. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.72.5.1422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S. (2012). Violation of the scalar property for time perception between 1 and 2 seconds: Evidence from interval discrimination, reproduction, and categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(4), 880–890. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027188

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grondin, S. (2014). About the (Non)scalar Property for Time Perception. In H. Merchant & V. de Lafuente (Eds.), Neurobiology of Interval Timing (Vol. 829, pp. 17–32). Springer New York. 10.1007/978-1-4939-1782-2_2

  • Haß, J., Blaschke, S., Rammsayer, T., & Herrmann, J. M. (2008). A neurocomputational model for optimal temporal processing. Journal of Computational Neuroscience, 25(3), 449–464. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10827-008-0088-4

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinton, S. C., & Rao, S. M. (2004). “One-thousand one … one-thousand two …”: Chronometric counting violates the scalar property in interval timing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11(1), 24–30. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206456

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holway, A. H., & Pratt, C. C. (1936). The Weber ratio for intensive discrimination. Psychological Review, 43(4), 322–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0059748

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamieson, D. G., & Petrusic, W. M. (1975). The dependence of time-order error direction on stimulus range. Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, 29(3), 175–182.

  • Killeen, P. R., & Weiss, N. A. (1987). Optimal timing and the Weber function. Psychological Review, 94(4), 455–468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.4.455

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kingdom, F., & Prins, N. (2016). Psychophysics: A Practical Introduction (2nd). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407156-8.12001-8

  • Lavoie, P., & Grondin, S. (2004). Information processing limitations as revealed by temporal discrimination. Brain and Cognition, 54(3), 198–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.039

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (1991). The comparative psychology of fixed-interval responding: Some quantitative analyses. Learning and Motivation, 22(1–2), 84–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(91)90018-4

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lejeune, H., & Wearden, J. H. (2006). Scalar Properties in Animal Timing: Conformity and Violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(11), 1875–1908. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600784649

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malapani, C., Rakitin, B., Levy, R., Meck, W. H., Deweer, B., Dubois, B., & Gibbon, J. (1998). Coupled Temporal Memories in Parkinson’s Disease: A Dopamine-Related Dysfunction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10(3), 316–331. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892998562762

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masin, S. (2009). The (Weber’s) law that never was. In M. Elliott & S. Antonijevic (Eds.), Fechner Day 2009: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 441–446). International Society for Psychophysics.

  • Matell, M. S., & Meck, W. H. (2000). Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. BioEssays: News and Reviews in Molecular, Cellular and Developmental Biology, 22(1), 94–103. 10.1002/(SICI)1521-1878(200001)22:1<94::AID-BIES14>3.0.CO;2-E

  • Millisecond. (2018). Inquisit (5.0) [Computer software]. Millisecond Software. https://www.millisecond.com

  • Navakatikyan, M. A. (2007). A model for residence time in concurrent variable interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87(1), 121–141.

  • Sowden, P. T. (2012). Psychophysics. In H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol 1: Foundations, planning, measures, and psychometrics. (pp. 445–458). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13619-024

  • Spiess, A.-N., & Neumeyer, N. (2010). An evaluation of R2 as an inadequate measure for nonlinear models in pharmacological and biochemical research: A Monte Carlo approach. BMC Pharmacology, 10(1), 6. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2210-10-6

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Steinberg. (2015). Cubase (8.5) [Computer software]. Steinberg Media Technologies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ueda, N., Maruo, K., & Sumiyoshi, T. (2018). Positive symptoms and time perception in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research. Cognition, 13, 3–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2018.07.002

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Wearden, J. H., & Lejeune, H. (2008). Scalar Properties in Human Timing: Conformity and Violations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(4), 569–587. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701282576

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodrow, H. (1930). The reproduction of temporal intervals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13(6), 473–499. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0070462

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

When one can detect a difference between two similar sounds it is called?

The just noticeable difference (JND), also known as the difference threshold, is the minimum level of stimulation that a person can detect 50% of the time.

How does Weber's law apply to difference threshold?

To quantify the difference threshold, psycho-physicist Ernst Weber developed what is known as the Weber's Law. Weber's Law states that rather than a constant, absolute amount of change, there must be a constant percentage change for two stimuli to be perceived as different.

What senses does Weber's law apply to?

Weber's Law can be applied to variety of sensory modalities (brightness, loudness, mass, line length, etc.). The size of the Weber fraction varies across modalities but in most cases tends to be a constant within a specific task modality.

What is the smallest detectable difference between two stimuli?

Just noticeable difference (JND): The smallest detectable difference between two stimuli, or the minimum change in a stimulus that can be correctly judged as different from a reference stimulus; also known as difference threshold.